Prévia do material em texto
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/229193473 The effects of group size on grazing in sheep Article in Applied Animal Behaviour Science · July 1993 DOI: 10.1016/0168-1591(93)90103-V CITATIONS 106 READS 62 5 authors, including: Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects: Foraging behavior View project Impacts of climate change on other invertebrates View project A. J. Parsons Massey University 182 PUBLICATIONS 6,516 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Jonathan A Newman Wilfrid Laurier University 152 PUBLICATIONS 5,831 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Robert J Orr Rothamsted Research, North Wyke, United Kingdom 178 PUBLICATIONS 2,631 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE All content following this page was uploaded by Jonathan A Newman on 21 October 2017. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/229193473_The_effects_of_group_size_on_grazing_in_sheep?enrichId=rgreq-0ebad49e133e7ec11bb8585ca0e09816-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIyOTE5MzQ3MztBUzo1NTIwMDgxNzM4NTQ3MjBAMTUwODYyMDQxMjAwMw%3D%3D&el=1_x_2&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/publication/229193473_The_effects_of_group_size_on_grazing_in_sheep?enrichId=rgreq-0ebad49e133e7ec11bb8585ca0e09816-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIyOTE5MzQ3MztBUzo1NTIwMDgxNzM4NTQ3MjBAMTUwODYyMDQxMjAwMw%3D%3D&el=1_x_3&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/project/Foraging-behavior?enrichId=rgreq-0ebad49e133e7ec11bb8585ca0e09816-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIyOTE5MzQ3MztBUzo1NTIwMDgxNzM4NTQ3MjBAMTUwODYyMDQxMjAwMw%3D%3D&el=1_x_9&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/project/Impacts-of-climate-change-on-other-invertebrates?enrichId=rgreq-0ebad49e133e7ec11bb8585ca0e09816-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIyOTE5MzQ3MztBUzo1NTIwMDgxNzM4NTQ3MjBAMTUwODYyMDQxMjAwMw%3D%3D&el=1_x_9&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/?enrichId=rgreq-0ebad49e133e7ec11bb8585ca0e09816-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIyOTE5MzQ3MztBUzo1NTIwMDgxNzM4NTQ3MjBAMTUwODYyMDQxMjAwMw%3D%3D&el=1_x_1&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/A_Parsons?enrichId=rgreq-0ebad49e133e7ec11bb8585ca0e09816-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIyOTE5MzQ3MztBUzo1NTIwMDgxNzM4NTQ3MjBAMTUwODYyMDQxMjAwMw%3D%3D&el=1_x_4&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/A_Parsons?enrichId=rgreq-0ebad49e133e7ec11bb8585ca0e09816-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIyOTE5MzQ3MztBUzo1NTIwMDgxNzM4NTQ3MjBAMTUwODYyMDQxMjAwMw%3D%3D&el=1_x_5&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/institution/Massey_University?enrichId=rgreq-0ebad49e133e7ec11bb8585ca0e09816-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIyOTE5MzQ3MztBUzo1NTIwMDgxNzM4NTQ3MjBAMTUwODYyMDQxMjAwMw%3D%3D&el=1_x_6&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/A_Parsons?enrichId=rgreq-0ebad49e133e7ec11bb8585ca0e09816-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIyOTE5MzQ3MztBUzo1NTIwMDgxNzM4NTQ3MjBAMTUwODYyMDQxMjAwMw%3D%3D&el=1_x_7&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Jonathan_Newman?enrichId=rgreq-0ebad49e133e7ec11bb8585ca0e09816-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIyOTE5MzQ3MztBUzo1NTIwMDgxNzM4NTQ3MjBAMTUwODYyMDQxMjAwMw%3D%3D&el=1_x_4&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Jonathan_Newman?enrichId=rgreq-0ebad49e133e7ec11bb8585ca0e09816-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIyOTE5MzQ3MztBUzo1NTIwMDgxNzM4NTQ3MjBAMTUwODYyMDQxMjAwMw%3D%3D&el=1_x_5&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/institution/Wilfrid_Laurier_University?enrichId=rgreq-0ebad49e133e7ec11bb8585ca0e09816-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIyOTE5MzQ3MztBUzo1NTIwMDgxNzM4NTQ3MjBAMTUwODYyMDQxMjAwMw%3D%3D&el=1_x_6&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Jonathan_Newman?enrichId=rgreq-0ebad49e133e7ec11bb8585ca0e09816-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIyOTE5MzQ3MztBUzo1NTIwMDgxNzM4NTQ3MjBAMTUwODYyMDQxMjAwMw%3D%3D&el=1_x_7&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Robert_Orr3?enrichId=rgreq-0ebad49e133e7ec11bb8585ca0e09816-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIyOTE5MzQ3MztBUzo1NTIwMDgxNzM4NTQ3MjBAMTUwODYyMDQxMjAwMw%3D%3D&el=1_x_4&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Robert_Orr3?enrichId=rgreq-0ebad49e133e7ec11bb8585ca0e09816-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIyOTE5MzQ3MztBUzo1NTIwMDgxNzM4NTQ3MjBAMTUwODYyMDQxMjAwMw%3D%3D&el=1_x_5&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Robert_Orr3?enrichId=rgreq-0ebad49e133e7ec11bb8585ca0e09816-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIyOTE5MzQ3MztBUzo1NTIwMDgxNzM4NTQ3MjBAMTUwODYyMDQxMjAwMw%3D%3D&el=1_x_7&_esc=publicationCoverPdf https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Jonathan_Newman?enrichId=rgreq-0ebad49e133e7ec11bb8585ca0e09816-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIyOTE5MzQ3MztBUzo1NTIwMDgxNzM4NTQ3MjBAMTUwODYyMDQxMjAwMw%3D%3D&el=1_x_10&_esc=publicationCoverPdf Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 37 ( 1993 ) 101-109 0168-1591/93/$06.00 © 1993 - Elsevier Science Publishers B.V. All rights reserved 101 The effects of group size on grazing time in sheep P.D. Penning *,a, A.J. Parsons a, J.A. Newman b, R.J. Orr ~, A. H a r v e y a aAFRC Institute of Grassland and Environmental Research, North Wyke Research Station, Okehampton EX20 2SB, UK bAFRC Unit of Ecology and Behaviour, Department of Zoology, University c~lO.xj~rd, South Parks Road, Oxford OXI 3PS, UK (Accepted 8 March 1993) Abstract Voluntary intake of herbage by grazing sheep (Ovis aries L. ) is the result of several interacting behaviours. In gregarious animals, such as sheep, the social environment may strongly influence some of the components of ingestive behaviour that control food intake. This study investigated the effects of group size on grazing time, one of the main components of ingestive behaviour governing daily herbage intake. Grazing time was recorded for focal sheep within groups, ranging in size from one to 15 over periods of 24 h. The animals were continuously stocked on a perennial ryegrass pasture ( Lol- ium perenne L. cv Parcour) maintained at a sward surface height of 6 cm. Groups were established 1 week prior to recording the behaviour of a focal animal in each grazing group. Recordings were made on three occasions for 24 h, with a period of 24 h between each recording. It was found that sheep in smaller groups spent less time grazing than sheep in larger groups. There was no relationship between group size and intra-meal intervals, prehension biting rate or number of meals, but animals in groups of one and two tended to have shorter meals than those in larger groups. There was indirect evidence. from measurements of changes in sward surface height, to suggest that intake of herbage was also reduced for animals grazing in groups of less than four. It was concluded that a minimum flock size of three sheep, but preferably four, is required for studies of grazing behaviour. Key words: Sheep; Grazing; Biting rate; Feeding Introduction Ef f i c i en t a n i m a l p r o d u c t i o n d e p e n d s o n a d e q u a t e levels o f v o l u n t a r y i n t a k e ( F o r b e s , 1986 ). T h i s s t a t e m e n t i m p l i e s t h a t a m a j o r l i m i t i n g f a c t o r in s h e e p p r o d u c t i o n is t he i r i nges t i ve b e h a v i o u r . T h i s c o m p r i s e s t h r e e s e p a r a t e c o m - p o n e n t s : g r a z i n g t i m e , b i t e m a s s a n d p r e h e n s i o n b i t i n g rate . A n i m a l sc ien t i s t s h a v e s p e n t m a n y yea r s a s sess ing f a c t o r s t h a t a f fec t t hese c o m p o n e n t s a n d h e n c e v o l u n t a r y f eed i n t a k e ( see r e v i e w b y F o r b e s , 1986 ) . A t y p i c a l v o l u n - t a r y f eed i n t a k e s t u d y w o u l d be c o n d u c t e d i n d o o r s u s i n g i n d i v i d u a l l y p e n n e d a n i m a l s t h a t a re o f f e r e d f eed ad l i b i t u m (e.g. W e s t o n , 1 9 8 9 ) . T h e s e s tud i e s a re c o n d u c t e d w i t h t he e x p e c t a t i o n t h a t t he k n o w l e d g e o f f a c to r s t h a t c o n t r o l v o l u n t a r y i n t a k e c a n be a p p l i e d genera l ly , so t h a t levels o f p r o d u c t i o n in re- *Corresponding author. 102 P.D. Penning et al./Appl. A nim. Behav. Sci. 37(1993) 101-109 lation to intake and genetic potential can be predicted and manipulated. However, the application of classical theories of intake control, developed in this manner, to grazing animals, may be vitiated by many other factors that cannot be taken into account in indoor feeding studies. Principal among these must be social interactions, because sheep are typically classified as social animals. For instance, McClymont (1967) reported social facilitation of phagic behaviour in sheep, and Rook and Penning ( 1991 ) found that sheep tend to be synchronous in their start of grazing bouts. This study investigated the effects of group size on grazing time, one of the major factors controlling daily intake. Animals, materials and methods In September 1991, group sizes of I to 10, 12 and 15 of mature, non-lactat- ing, non-pregnant Scottish Half bred ewes, with a mean liveweight of 84.7 _+ 1.29 kg, were established. The groups were maintained on monocul- tures of perennial ryegrass (Lolium perenne L. cv Parcour) at a sward surface height (SSH) of 6 cm (Bircham, 1981 ) with a constant area of 0.03 ha per animal. None of the plots containing the animals were contiguous and there was a m in imum distance between plots of 5 m. The plot boundaries were constructed from wire mesh and electric fencing. It was not possible to visu- ally isolate the groups of animals. These groups remained together for 1 week in order to become accustomed to their group members and group sizes. All animals had access to water and no supplementary feed was offered. After 1 week, one sheep (at random) from each group was fitted with a behaviour recorder. This technique has been described in detail by Penning (1983) and Penning et al. (1984). Briefly, gnathometers were used to pro- duce analogue electrical signals of the jaw movements that were recorded on miniature tape recorders, carried on the ewes, over a 24-h period commenc- ing at 10:00 h. These signals were subsequently replayed and the waveforms analysed by computer to give time spent grazing, ruminating and idling and to count the jaw movements associated with these activities. The data were summarised on a minute by minute basis and an animal was deemed to be either eating, ruminating or idling if it spent at least 30 s in any 1 min per- forming one of these behaviours. To fit or remove grazing behaviour equip- ment required 5 min per animal when the animals were restrained in small pens adjacent to each plot. Otherwise, the animals were left undisturbed on an area of pasture that was isolated from roads and tracks, and were only visited by the experimenters. Group size 1 was replicated three times and group size 2 was replicated twice. However, lack of land, animals and behav- iour recording equipment prevented replication of the larger groups. For each group size, the behaviour of one focal animal each day was recorded on Days 8, 10 and 12 after the groups were formed. A different focal animal was cho- P.D. Penning et al./Appl. Anirn. Behav. Sci. 37(1993) 101-109 103 sen at random each day. The weather was similar on the 3 days when the recordings were made. Individual animals within a group could not be con- sidered independent as the hypothesis to be tested was that, the number of individuals within a group affected the behaviour of the individuals within that group. In addition, Rook and Penning ( 1991 ) have demonstrated that sheep within a group do not behave independently. Therefore a mean of the three measurements made at each group size, was calculated and the data were analysed by regression using group size as the independent variate. Ad- justed R2 (proportion of the variance accounted for;/~2) was calculated for each of the regression models fitted. Twenty-five measurements of SSH were recorded on each plot at the start and end of the experiment. Results Fig. 1 shows the asymptotic relationship between grazing time and group size. The equation calculated was: grazing time (min h - ] ) = 629 - 311" exp [ - 0.46* group size ), n = 12, R2=0.48. Linear and quadratic regressions were also fitted to the data and/~2 was 0.28 and 0.43, respectively. The asymptotic relationship is preferred as it ac- counts for a greater proportion of the variance than the linear or quadratic 8 0 0 7 0 0 - # -~ 600 ~ 500 4 0 0 3 0 0 ~ ' 0 5 1 0 15 G r o u p s i z e Fig. 1. Grazing time (min 24 h ]) as a function of group size. The points are mean values fbr three animals. (- exponential fit; ...... piecewise regression with the break point at group size 3 and - - piecewise regression with the break point at group size 4 ). 104 P.D. Penning et aL /Appl. Anita. Behav. Sci. 37(1993) 101-109 regressions and, in addition, it is biologically meaningful. That is a group would be expected to contain some m i n i m u m critical number of animals and, above this group size, grazing t ime would remain constant, provided the area of pasture per animal also remains constant, as was the case in this experi- ment. An asymptotic curve describes this type of response, whereas a qua- dratic relationship would predict a decrease in grazing time for large groups of animals. Piecewise linear regression analysis was also carried out (Draper and Smith, 1981 ) and break points were systematically fitted for group sizes 2 to 8 to determine the m in im um group size at which grazing time was not affected. The greatest variance accounted for was achieved when the break point (see Fig. 1 ) was set at a group size 3 (/~2 =0.50, n = 12) which corresponded to 587 min of grazing in 24 h. Animals in groups of one or two grazed for 164 min and 82 min less 24 h-~, respectively. The slope of the regression up to the break point was 82 ( + 31.0) min per animal in the group. The slope of the regression from the break point to group size 15 was 5 ( + 5.0) min per animal in the group and was not significantly different from zero. Setting the break point to four (see Fig. 1 ) did not significantly reduce the variance ac- counted for (_g2 = 0.49, n = 12), however, using group size 5 as the break point reduced the variance accounted for/~a = 0.40, n = 12 ). The/~2 values show that group size only accounted for about 50% of the variation in grazing time. The replication in this experiment was achieved by recording grazing t ime on three separate days. To examine whether differ- ences between days could explain some of the variability in the data, an anal- ysis of variance on the mean grazing time, across groups, was carried out con- sidering day as the treatment (n = 12 ). No significant difference between days was found (P=0 .25 ) and the mean values were: 595, 612 and 514 (s.e.d. 43.7 ) min 24 h -1. It was not possible to analyse for interactions between day and group size as only one focal animal was used within a group on each day, as previously discussed. Figure 2 shows a comparison of the patterns of grazing between group size 1 and group size 15. It shows that in any given hour animals in a group of 15 are likely to have grazed for a longer period of time than those kept individually. This result is particularly obvious for the period of darkness (20:00-06:00 h ). The effects of group size on bouts of grazing ( 'meals ') was also analysed using the method described by Forbes (1986) and Sibly et al. (1990) to de- termine intra- and inter-bout intervals. These authors showed that the distri- butionof pauses during eating comprises two populations, those that occur within an eating bout (intra-bout intervals) and those that occur between eating bouts (inter-bout intervals). The distribution of pauses in grazing was analysed to determine the inter-bout interval. Piecewise regression analysis was applied to the natural logarithm of the frequency of each pause duration. P.D. Penning et al./Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 37 (1993) 101-109 [ 05 60 i I 5O =~ 40 ~_ 20 1 1 0 0 ~ ~ C~ 1 0 : 0 0 1 4 : 0 0 1 8 : 0 0 2 2 : 0 0 0 2 : 0 0 T i m e ( h ) 0 6 : 0 0 Fig. 2. Patterns of grazing in group sizes 1 • and g~ 15. This figure shows the number of minutes spent grazing mean for three animals) in each hour for each group. 6 " o>, 5!" 4 ~ 3 ' ~ • O O • 0 2 • 1 . . . . t • • • • o o o 0 I _ _ u = = ~ . _ $ j 0 1 0 2 0 3O 4O Interval ( m i n ) Fig. 3. Log frequency of intervals during grazing plotted against length of interval. The fitted regression lines show the change from intra- to inter-bout interval occurred at 6 rain. The frequency d i s tr ibut ions o f pause lengths did not vary be tween group sizes so all data were p o o l e d ( 2 4 h recordings for 36 a n i m a l s ) and are s h o w n for intervals from 1 to 30 m i n in Fig. 3. The result o f this analysis suggested that 6 min was the length o f the inter-bout interval On(frequency)=5.7-0.6* 106 P.D. Penning et aL/Appl. Anirn. Behav. Sci. 37 (1993) 101-109 Table 1 The effects of group size on prehension biting rates, number of grazing bouts of 6 min length and more and duration of bouts (means for three animals) Group size Prehension rate Number of Length of (number of animals ) (bi tes/min ) grazing bouts bouts (min) 24 h - l 1 79 7.3 62 2 70 6.3 72 3 68 4.3 148 4 102 6.3 114 5 75 9.0 69 6 77 6.7 105 7 97 7.0 84 8 62 6.5 114 9 84 4.0 169 l0 76 5.3 116 12 73 7.0 103 15 79 7.0 97 c & c z:: © -1 r i 2 0 L 5 10 1 5 Group size Fig. 4. The effects o f group size on the change in SSH (cm) . (interval min); for interval 6 min; K 2 = 0 . 8 4 ) . Using intervals > 6 rain as the inter-bout duration, numbers of bouts and bout durations were calculated for each group size (Table 1 ). It can be seen from Table 1 that the number o f meals in 24 h was not correlated with group size, but the mean length o f each bout tends to be shorter for group size 1 or 2 than for larger groups. Table 1 also shows that the mean prehension biting P.D. Penning et al./AppL Anim. Behav. Sci. 37(1993) 101-109 1 07 rate (b i tes /min grazing) was not correlated with group size. Since bite mass was not measured, it is not possible to estimate intake rate and thus total daily intake. However, since the SSH and stocking densities were the same across groups, it seems unlikely that there would be marked differences in intake rate between groups (Penning et al., 1991 a). Therefore, a decrease in grazing time seems likely to have resulted in a lower daily intake of herbage. This is supported, indirectly, by the SSH data (Fig. 4), which shows that there was a smaller decrease in SSH from group sizes less than four compared with larger groups. This implies that less herbage was consumed by animals in the rela- tively small groups. Discussion Although this study has little direct practical application for grazing man- agement, it should be important to researchers who study voluntary feed in- take or diet choice in sheep. Many researchers use single sheep, individually penned as units of replication (e.g. Illius et al., 1992; Newman et al., 1992) although Marsden and Wood-Gush (1986) and Done-Currie et al. (1984) have questioned the validity of the general extrapolation of results from such animals. This study shows that sheep kept individually at pasture may not behave in the same way as when they graze as a member of a flock. The results suggest that the min im um unit of replication for grazing experiments should be groups of, at least, group size 4 or 5, but certainly not less than 3. These results are also supported by those of Southcott et al. ( 1962 ), who found that animals maintained in smaller group sizes gained less weight than those maintained in larger group sizes. It is not known why sheep should have longer grazing times in larger flocks and it may have been the result of social facilitation. However, Rook and Penning ( 1991 ) found that sheep in groups tended to synchronise the start of their grazing bouts, but were less synchronised at the end of these bouts. Hence, bout duration does not seem to be a result of social facilitation. Also, Benham (1984) only identified social facilitation for movement , but not for feeding in grazing dairy cows. Another possibility is that it is an ancestral behaviour correlated with the level of perceived danger of predation. Many animal spe- cies flock for anti-predator benefits and individual sheep may perceive them- selves to be under greater threat than they might be as part of a group, owing to the 'Dilution effect' (Pulliam and Caraco, 1984). In addition, animals that graze in groups may benefit from the increased number of individuals that are being vigilant, the so called 'many eyes' hypothesis (Pulliam, 1973 ). Many other social animals show this same effect of group size on feeding time in studies where the results can be more readily related to apparent danger of predation (see Lima and Dill, 1990, for review). 108 P.D. Penning et aL/Appk Anim. Behav. Sci. 37(1993) 101-109 This experiment can do tittle to resolve these two possibilities, but it does suggest that group size may be a critical variable in determining grazing be- haviour and should perhaps be considered in other contexts, e.g. in under- standing the mechanisms controlling daily intake. In attempts to understand factors controlling daily intake by grazing rumi- nants studies using 'sward boards' (Black and Kenney, 1984), grazing cages (Burlison et al., 1991 ) and turves (Illius and Gordon, 1990; Newman et al., 1992) have provided valuable information on the mechanisms and physical constraints to intake rate (e.g. the interrelationships between sward density, bite mass, intake rate etc.). These experiments, by their nature, last at the most for 1 or 2 min and cannot therefore address the mechanisms controlling grazing time. Animals in many circumstances control daily intake by adjust- ing grazing time, often completely compensating for low intake rates (Pen- ning et al., 1991 b ). The understanding of intake rate alone cannot, therefore, be extended to understand how total intake is controlled. Ecologists studying wild animals have had some success in addressing total daily feeding time by considering it as a behavioural decision rather than, necessarily, a constraint. They consider that this behaviour may be shaped by natural selection, such that the consequences of an increase in grazing time, must be weighed against the consequences of engaging in alternative behaviours (e.g. avoiding preda- tion). This 'Darwinian fitness' approach can provide a non-mechanistic hy- pothesis regarding the 'limitations' on grazing time (i.e. that it is an adaptive behaviour rather than a mechanistic constraint). Illustrations of this ap- proach are seen in Mangel and Clark (1986), McNamara and Houston (1986), and Newman (1990). The present paper clearly indicates that, for whatever motivation, sheep do use behavioural cues to modify grazing time. Acknowledgements We thank R.A. Champion, S. Da Costa and R.H. Johnson for assistance in the field and Dr A.J. Rook for statistical advice. This research was supported by a grant from the Agricultural and Food Research Council's Joint Agricul- ture and Environment Programme. The authors also thank Prof. J.R.Kreb- sand and Dr A. Lawrence for useful comments on earlier versions of this manuscript. References Benham, P.F.J., 1984. Social organisation in groups of cattle and the inter-relationships be- tween social and grazing behaviours under different grazing management systems. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Reading, 213 pp. Bircham, J.S., 1981. Herbage growth and utilization under continuous stocking management. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Edinburgh, 379 pp. PD. Penning et al./Appl. Anirn. Behav. Sci. 37(1993) 101-109 1 09 Black, J.L. and Kenney, P.A., 1984. Factors affecting diet selection by sheep. 1I. Height and density of pasture. Aust. J. Agric. Res., 35: 565-578. Burlison, A.J., Hodgson, J. and Illius, A.W., 1991. Sward canopy structure and the bite dimen- sion and bite weight of grazing sheep. Grass Forage Sci., 46: 29-38. Done-Currie, J.R., Hecker, J.F. and Wodzicka-Tomaszewska, M., 1984. Behaviour of sheep transferred from pasture to an animal house. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci., 12:121-130 Draper, N.R. and Smith, H., 1981. Applied regression analysis. 2nd Edn. John Wiley. New York, pp. 250-257. Forbes, J.M., 1986. The voluntary food intake of farm animals. Butterworth, Leeds, UK, 206 Pp. lllius, A.W., Clark, O.A. and Hodgson, J., 1992. Discrimination and patch choice by sheep graz- ing grass-clover swards. J. Anim. Ecol., 61: 183-194. lllius, A.W. and Gordon, I.A., 1990. Constraints on diet selection and foraging behaviour in mammalian herbivores. In: R.N.Hughes (Editor), Behavioural Mechanisms of Food Selec- tion. NATO ASI Series. Springer, Berlin, pp. 369-392. Lima, S.L. and Dill, L.M., 1990. Behavioural decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can. J. Zool., 69:619-640. Mangel, M. and Clark, C.W., 1986. Towards a unified foraging theory. Ecology, 67:1127-1138. Marsden, D. and Wood-Gush, D.G.M., 1986. A note on the behaviour of individually-penned sheep regarding their use for research purposes. Anim. Prod., 42:157-159. McClymont, G.L., 1967. Selectivity and intake in the grazing ruminant. In: Code, C.F. (Edi- tor), Handbook of Physiology, Section 6, Vol. I. Am. Physiol. Soc., Washington, DC. pp. 129-137. McNamara, J.M. and Houston, A.I., 1986. The common currency for behavioural decisions. Am. Nat., 127: 358-378. Newman, J.A., 1990. Patch use under predation hazard: foraging behaviour in a simple sto- chastic environment. Oikos, 61: 29-44. Newman, J.A., Parsons, A.J. and Harvey, A., 1992. Not all sheep prefer clover diet: selection revisited. J. Agric. Sci., 119: 275-283. Penning, P.D., 1983. A technique to record automatically some aspects of grazing and ruminat- ing behaviour in sheep. Grass Forage Sci., 38: 89-96. Penning, P.D., Parsons, A.J., Orr, R.J. and Treacher, T.T., 1991a. Intake and behaviour re- sponses by sheep to changes in sward characteristics under continuous stocking. Grass For- age Sci., 46:15-28. Penning, P.D., Rook, A.J. and Orr, R.J., 199 lb. Patterns of ingestive behaviour of sheep contin- uously stocked on monocultures of ryegrass or white clover. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci.. 31: 237-250. Penning, P.D., Steel, G.L. and Johnson, R.H., 1984. Further development and use of an auto- matic recording system in sheep grazing studies. Grass Forage Sci., 39:345-351. Pulliam, H.R., 1973. On the advantages of flocking. J. Theor. Biol., 38: 419-422. Pulliam, H.R. and Caraco, T., 1984. Living in groups: is there an optimal group size? In: J.R. Krebs and N.B. Davies (Editors), Behavioural Ecology. Blackwell Scientific, Oxford, pp. 122-147. Rook, A.J and Penning, P.D., 1991. Synchronisation of eating, ruminating and idling activity by grazing sheep. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci., 32:157-166. Sibly, R.M. Nott, H.M.R. and Fletcher, D.J., 1990. Splitting behaviour into bouts. Anim. Be- hav., 39: 63-69. Southcott, W.H., Roe, R. and Turner, H.N., 1962. Grazing management of native pastures in the New England region of New South Wales. Aust. J. Agri. Res., 13: 880-893. Weston, R.H., 1989. Factors limiting the intake of feed by sheep, xv. Voluntary feed consump- tion and digestion in lambs fed chopped roughage diets varying in quality. Aust. J. Agric. Res., 40:643-661. V i e w p u b l i c a t i o n s t a t sV i e w p u b l i c a t i o n s t a t s https://www.researchgate.net/publication/229193473