Prévia do material em texto
This article was downloaded by: [UQ Library] On: 16 November 2014, At: 20:05 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Journal of Clinical Child Psychology Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hcap19 Attention-Deficit Disorder With (ADD/H) and Without (ADD/WO) Hyperactivity: Behavioral and Neuropsychological Differentiation Patricia Goodyear & George W. Hynd Published online: 07 Jun 2010. To cite this article: Patricia Goodyear & George W. Hynd (1992) Attention-Deficit Disorder With (ADD/H) and Without (ADD/ WO) Hyperactivity: Behavioral and Neuropsychological Differentiation, Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 21:3, 273-305, DOI: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2103_9 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp2103_9 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http:// www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hcap19 http://www.tandfonline.com/action/showCitFormats?doi=10.1207/s15374424jccp2103_9 http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp2103_9 http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions Journal olF Clinical Child Psychology 1992, Vol. 21, No. 3, 273-305 Copyright 1992 by Lawrence Erlhaum Associates, Inc. rlttention-Deficit Disorder With (ADD/H) and Without (AIC)D/WO) Hyperactivity: Behavioral and Neuropsychological Diffprentiation Patricia Goodyear Departments of Education and Psychology, Furman University George W. Hynd Departments of Special Education and Psychology, U~rsiversity of Georgia and Neurology, Medical College of Georgia Reviewed behavioral and neuropsychological studies addressing attentiop-dejicit disorder with (ADD/H) and without (ADD/WO) hyperactivity. Revie$ sf the behavioral studies suggests that children with ADD/H have more behavi?r prob- lems, are less popular, are more self-destructive, and are more likely t4 have a codiagnosis of conduct disorder. Children with ADD/ WO seem more socia fly with- drawn, have a slower cognitive tempo, are more self-conscious, ana! have p higher incidence of developmental learning disorders. Neuropsychological stwdieq suggest that children with ADD/H or ADD/ WO may dztfer electrophysio1o~:ically f nd that ADD/WO children may have defcits in autonzaticity similar to childten with learning disabilities. The literature provides more support for the diagnosti nonzen- cloture found in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disor1ers (3rd ed.; DSM-III; American Psychiatric Association, 1980) than for the pblythetic approach characterizing the diagnosis of attention-defcit hyperactivity flisonder (ADHD) in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disolrd~?rs Urd ed., rev.; DSM-III-R; American Psychiatric Association, 1987). Aniong the most common behavior disorders of children and adolescents are problems related to hy- peractivity and inattention. The American Psychiat- ric Association (1987) in the Diagnostic and Statisti- cal Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd ed., rev.; DSM--III--R) estimated that the disorders may occur in as many as 3% of children. Barkley (1982), after an extensive review of the literature, estimated prevallence at 3% to 5% of school-age children and suggested that these problems comprise 30% to 40% of the total referrals to chid guidance clinics. Based on these estimates, then, it would be expected that at least one child in every classroom could be so identified. Diagnostic Nomenclature In the earlier second edition of the manual (DSM-II; American Psychiatric Association, 1968), the disorder was known as hyperkinetic reac- tion qf childhood. Diagnostic emphasis was on the high activity level with little emphasis on symptoms Requests for reprints should be sent to George W. Hynd, Center Tor Clinical and Developmental Neuropsychology, EXC- Aderhold Hall, University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602. of inattention. The pulblicatihn of the third edition (DSA4-III;. Americain ~s+hiarfric Association, 1980) resulted in a controvers/al change in the classi- fication which renamed, red+finecl, and subdivided the previous category. 'The tedm attention defcit dis- order (ADD) was introduce/d to replace hyperki- netic reaction of childhood dnd signified a shift in diagnostic emphasis from a dibturbance of activity to a disturbance of attention (qarlson, 1986 Douglas & Peters, 1979; Lahey, ~cldau~lhenc~, Strauss, & Frame, 1984). The suk~divisiqn of the category into attention deficits with (ADI)/H:) and without hy- peractivity (ADDlWO) fu ther highlighted this change of emphasis as it b a a e polssible to diagnose level. F impairment of attentioin withgut heightened activity I Following the introducti'bn of ADD/WO in 1980, the subtyping of atten ion deficits was criti- I cized as empirically unfoundqd @.:couth, 1986). Such criticism led to the removal of ADD/WO from DSM-III-R in 1987. This latest diagnostic and classification system has subqtitutced two disorders, attention-deficit hyperactivity +liiso~~iier (ADHD) and undzrerentiated attention-defScit d,iorder OJADD), for the previous ADD categoiy. According to New- Corn et al. (1989), almost two thirds of the ADHD D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 symptoms enumerated in DSM-III-R describe behaviors reflective of hyperactivity, impulsivity, or both; inattentive behaviors are referred to specifi- cally in only approximately one third of the symp- toms. The current ADHD category shifts diagnostic emphasis away from attention disturbance. At the same time, the UADD category maintains that there may be "disturbances in which the predominant fea- ture is the persistence of developmentally inappro- priate and marked inattention that is not a symptom of another disorder" (American Psychiatric Associ- ation, 1987, p. 95). Thus, DSM-III-R has removed the ADD/WO terminology but has retained the concept that an attention disorder may occur in the absence of hyperactivity. Although the concept of an attention disorder in the absence of hyperactivity has been retained, its diagnosis has been altered. A D D N O and UADD are not equivalent categories (Lahey, Schaughency, Hynd, Carlson, & Nieves, 1987). A D D N O diagno- sis (DSM-III) requires both inattention and impul- sivity whereas UADD (DSM-III-R) requires only inattention without further delineation of diagnostic criteria. Further complicating the diagnosis of attention disorders is the "polythetic" (Cantwell & Baker, 1988; Lahey et al., 1987) approqch to diagnosisof ADHD (Hynd, Lorys, et al., 1991; Lahey et al., 1988; Lahey et al., 1987). This polythetic approach requires the presence of at least 8 of 14 possible symptoms associated with ADHD's "essential fea- tures" (i.e., inattention, impulsiveness, hyperac- tivity; American Psychiatric Association, 1987). With this approach, the diagnosis of ADHD may be made without the requirement that. symptoms from each of the essential features be prfisent, an apparent acknowledgment of the difficulty of separating hy- peractivity and impulsivity characteristics (Lahey et al., 1988; Newcosn et al., 1984). The effect of this, however, is that any combination of symptoms of inattention and/or irnpulsivenesg and/or hyperac- tivity can result in an APHD diagnosis. As Ostrom and Jenson (1988) pointed out, "While the narrative description of ADHD in DSM-In-R highlights attentional problems, it wopld be theoretically possi- ble to make the diagnosis without considering any of the attention items" (p. 263), Table 1 summarizes the diagnostic, erite?ria found in DSM-111 and DSM- III- R. As predicted, Meworn et al. (1989) identified a group of nonreferred students meeti~g ADHD crite- ria who displayed hyperactive and impulsive but not necessarily inattentive behaviors. Based on this finding, the authors ooncluded that ADHD criteria allow identification of a more heterogeneous group than A D D m ariteria. August and Garfinkel (1989) also found heterogeneity with the ADHD criteria. With a nonreferred sample, they identified two inat- tentive-hyperactive groups; 80% exhibited behav- ioral difficulties and 20% exhibited cognitive diffi- culties such as reading disabilities. Both the failure to delineate UADD criteria and the polythetic nature of the ADHD classification affect the diagnosis of attention deficits in the ab- sence of hyperactivity. It is now likely that because of the combined presence of inattention and impul- sivity required in ADD/WO diagnosis (American Pychiatric Association, 1980) many A D D N O children will be subsumed under the ADHD poly- thetic criteria (Lahey et al., 1988; Lahey et al., 1987). In fact, in a recent study, Barkley, DuPaul, and McMurray (1990) found that 31% of their ADDJWO group also met ADHD criteria. Having diagnostic criteria that yield such heterogeneous groups (using the DSM-III-R) creates further confusion regarding the possible presence of ADD subtypes and may be unwarranted because the change in nomenclature occurred at a time when empirical evidence was accumulating in support of behavioral differences between ADD/H and ADDlWO children (Brown, 1986; Cantwell, 1984; Cantwell & Baker, 1988; Carlson, Lahey, & Neeper, 1986; Dykman, Ackerman, & McCray, 1980; Edalbro~k~ Costellp, & Kessler, 1984; King & Young, 1982; Lqhey, Schaughency, Frame, & Strauss, 1985; Lahey et al., 1987; Lahey et al., 1984). Continued concern over the ADD subty- ping issue is raected in the call of the American Psychiatric Asslociation (1987) for researcb into the validity and appropriate Qa#ifi$ition of the poorly de- lineated UADQ aategory. This concern is intensi- fied by the fact that the fourth edition of the man- ual (DSM-I;C") is sqon ta be published, In this cantext then, the purpose of thie article is to evaluate critioally thaliterature regarding the be- havioral diSferer~$iation of ADD subtypes as delin- eated ia DSM-111 in 198B The importance of this issue is undoncr~rqd by recent research thslt has sug- gested empiriaid links, bqtwem children with ADD/ WQ and learning disabilitiaa (Hynd, Larys, et al., 1991) and between children with ADD& and co- morbid disorders assaciacted with externalizing behaviors (e,g,, cond~@t disorder, oppositional defi- ant disorder; Lahey et d., 1987). Respite the, pvblichtion of DSM-III-R in 1987, it has taken a deosldle far sufficient research to be published whiclh addresi$es adequately the behav- ioral and nle~rcupsy~halogi~~l differentiation of ADDM and &S)D/WO. This literature should provide the basis for some conclusions as to the theoretical and clinical relevance of the notion that ADD subtypes exist. A second purpose af this article is to review the D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 SUBTYPES OF ADD Table 1. Criteria for Diagnosing ADDs Criteria for ADD/H ADD/WO ADHD UADD Diagnosis (DSM-III) (DSM-IIZ) (DSM-111--R) (DSM-111-R) -- Inattention Impulsivity Hyperactivity At least 8 of the following 14 symptolns X X are present: X (3) Is easily distracted by extraaeous stimuli. (6) Has difficulty following thropgh on instructions from others (not clue to oppositional behavior or failute of comprehension); e.g., fails to finish chores. (7) Has difficulty sustaining attehtion in tasks or play activities. (12) Often does not seem to listen to what is being said to him or her. (13) Often loses things necessary for tasks or activities at school od at home (e.g., toys, pencils, books, assignments). (4) Has difficulty awaiting Iturn in games X or group situations. (5) Often blurts out answers to questions before they have been complTed. (8) Often shifts from one uncompletrd activity to another. (9) Has difficulty playlng quietly. (1 1) Often interrupts or intrude$ on others; e.g., butts into other qhildren's games. (14) Often engages in physically dangerous activities without considering possible consequences. (1) Often fidgets with hands or feet or squirms in seat. (2) Has difficulty remaining seated when required to. (10) Often talks excessively. Onset before age 7 X X X Duration of art least 6 months X X X Not du~e to schizophrenia, affective disor~der, or severe or profound retardation X X Does not meet criteria for a pervasive developmental disorder Note: Numbers denote discriminating power in data from national field trial of DSM-111-R (American Psjchiatric Association, 1987) criteria. neuro~psychological studies concerning ADD/H and ADDIWO. Because ADDs presumably are the result of neurological dysfunction and the behav- ioral nature of these two subtypes may be different, it makes c~onceptual sense that neuropsychological differences may exist. Behavioral Studies of ADDS Presently, 21 behavioral studies have addressed whether children diagnosed with ADD/H or ADD/ WO can be differentiated; these are summarized in Table 2. This section atfdressets clirical and method- ological issues and sunimari~es tlhe results of these behavioral studies. Subject Characteristics ' Sample size. Overall, beihavl~oral studies have contrasted 692 ADD/H children with 481 ADD/ WO children, 344 normal controls, and 166 clinic controls. Average sample siqe for these studies is 32.9 for ADD/H and 22.9 for A,DD/WO. These averages, however, ar~e somewhist misleading be- D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 Table 2. Behavioral Studies of Children Diagnosed as Having ADD/H or ADD/ WO Author(s) N Differential Diagnosis Behavioral Measures Results Dykman et al. (1980) 43 males Co-Occurring Diagnoses: 1. Standard, double-blind, cross-over-de- 1. ADDAVO improved significantly more ADD/H: n = 15 All subjects referred for school-related aca- sign drug study with methyfphenidate. in search rate than ADD/H group. ADD/WO: n = 11 demic and behavioral difficulties. 2. Pribram task, measure of sustained atten- 2. ADD/WO subjects received lowest lev- Clinical Controls: Operational Criteria: tion scored for search and solution rates. els of methylphenidate but registered Reading disabled (RD): n = 1 1 1. Hyperactivity = Raw score 2 15 on greatest improvement in search rate. Reading disabled and hyperactive (RD/H): Abbreviated Teachers' Conners Raw Score ADD/H 22.3 RD/H 20.3 RD 6.4 A D D N O 7.3 and Parent Interview corroborating history of management problems. 2. FSIQ2 90. 3. Not on medication at time of initial ratings. Maurer & Stewart 297 subjects (225 male, 72 female) Co-Occurring Diagnoses: (1980) Meeting criteria for ADD = 166 5/9 LD ADD/H: n = 114 (95 male, 19 female) 3/9 encopretic Conduct disordered: n = 34 1/9 conduct disordered Depression: n = 3 Operational Criteria: Adjustment reaction: n = 1 1. ADD/WO = Autism: n = 2 a. History of problems with short at- Schizophrenia: n = 1 tention span. Hyperthyroidism: n = 1 b. Difficulty finishing school work or Undiagnosed: n = 2 projects. ADD/WO: n = 9 (7 male, 2 female) c. DSM-111 criterion of impulsive- None; study conducted on basis of retro- Chart review showed: spective chart review. 1. 5/9 rated as having short attention span. 2. 5/9 rated as lacking motivation. 3. 5/9 rated impatient. 4. 1/9 rated impulsive. 5. None rated as fighting. King & Young (1982) 58 males ADD/H: n = 22 A D D N O : n = 9 Normal controls: n = 27 ness not required. 2. No IQ criterion. 3. Status of medication not reported. Operational criteria: Teacher Perceptions: Teacher Perceptions (using Conners 1. SNAP Checklist-Teacher: ADD/H 1. Conners Teacher Rating Scale (39- Teacher Rating Scale): = rating of very much or pretty item version). 1. ADD/WO differed from control much or at least (a) 2 hyperactivity 2. Likert scale concerning academic group only on inattention. symptoms and (b) 3 inattention progress. 2. ADD/H and ADD/WO groups did symptoms. ADD/WO = at least Peer and Self-perceptions: not differ on Inattention factor. D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 Edelbrock et al. 87 males (1984) ADD/H: n = 18 ADD/WO: n = 7 Clinical controls: n = 62 one rating of pretty much or very much for both inattention and im- pulsivity combined; no ratings of very .-uc!: m d x~ mere thax gne rat ing of pretty much on hyperactivity symptoms. 2. Not on medication at time of ratings. 3. Average scaled score on WISCR vocabulary. Co-Occurring Diagnoses: No exclusions for LD/BD required. All sub- jects referred for mental health services. Operational Criteria: 1. Independent diagnostic agreement by two child psychiatrists reviewing case histories. 2. No IQ criterion. 3. Medication status unreported. Lahey et al. (1984) 50 subjects (36 male, 14 female) Co-Occurring Diagnoses: ADD/H: n = 10 (9 male, 1 female) Overlapping conduct disorders in 8/10 ADD/WO: n = 20 (13 male, 7 female) ADD/H subjects. Normal controls: n = 20 (14 male, 6 fe- Operational Criteria With RBPC: male) 1. All ADD subjects scored 2 1 stan- dard deviation above mean ( 2 12) on Attention Problem-Immaturity factor. 2. ADD/H also scored 2 1 standard deviation above mean ( 2 5) on Motor Excess factor. 3. No IQ criterion. 4. No report on status of medication. Rubenstein & Brown 46 subjects (39 male, 7 female) (1984) ADD/H: n = 23 ADD/WO: n = 23 Operational Criterion: Two GZpendent TaTers s se fved and rated subjects' behavior during interaction with staE member in structured, ciassroomiike, task-oriented setting. Kappa interrater reli- abilities: ADD/WO = .50 (fair to good); ADD/H = .37 (fair to poor) 1. Sociometric measure completed by entire class. 2. L ie r t scale to measure accuracy of . .nm&.roA ..r\...r1.3&+.. -..A o,.nAnm:n yL'-'.LU y"y"'U""j ','all '4""U.c.ll'r ability. 3. Preschcc! and Primary Self-Concept Scale. Teachers completed the Teacher Report Form of the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1983). 1. Teacher ratings on four RBPC factors which had not been used to identify the two experimental groups: a. Conduct Disorder. b. Socialized Aggression. c. Anxiety-Withdrawal. d. Psychotic Behaviors. 2. Classroom teachers: Likert scale of sports performance. 3. Physical education teachers: Likert scale of sports performance. 4. Sociometric and peer measures. 5. Self-report measures a. cnI. b. Piers-Harris test (P-H). Attention and Impulsivity Measures: Matchinz -Famifiaf Tigutes Test (MFF"ff children's Checking ask (CCT) Embedded Figures Test Attention-Concentration factor of WISC-R (arithmetic, digit span, coding) Academic Achievement: WRAT Detroit Tests 3. ADD/H > ADD/WO on hyperac- tivity. 4. ADD/H > ADD/WO on conduct ..rr\l.lnmc ""'L"'". 5. Findings suggest ADD/WO may be independent of conduct disorders. Peer and Self-Perceptions: Both hDD/H and ADD/WO children perceived more negatively by peers than were controls on sociometric measures. 1. ADD/WO > ADD/H: Social With- drawal. 2. ADD/H > ADD/WO: (a) Unpopular, (b) Self-Destructive, (c) Nervous-Over- active, and (d) Aggressive. 3. ADD/H > ADD/WO and control groups on behavior problems. 4. ADD/WO < ADD/H on current school performance. 5. ADD/WO > ADD/H in grade repeti- tions. 1. ADD/H r Control: a. Conduct Disorder. b. Socialized Aggression. c. Psychotic Behaviors. 2. ADD/N < Control on academics. 3. ADD/WO > Control on Anxiety-With- drawai. 4. ADD/WO < Control on academic and sports performance. In discriminant analyses, only TRSI re- tain&. the of TRSI to classify children as ADD/WO or ADD/H was only slightly - --- --- --L- .LA- ..--An- ..I,."":&."*:,.- IIIUIC aLLuIaLC luau l a u u u i u C~DUIIIC. ( ILIUII . (Continued) D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 Table 2. (Continued) Author(s) N Differential Diagnosis Berry et al. (1985) 228 subjects (164 male, 64 female) ADD/H: n = 94 (72 male, 22 female) ADD/WO: n = 40 (30 male, 10 female) Normal controls: n = 94 (62 male, 32 fe- male) Borden et al. (1985) Same sample as Rubenstein & Brown (1984). Nine subjects for whom complete data were not available were eliminated from analysis; therefore 37 subjects re- mained. Co-Occurring Diagnoses: Male Female Learning disability 63% 50% Reading disorder 53% 41% Math disorder 34% 25% Expressive language disorder 24% 28% Receptive language dis- order 7% 9% Exclusions: 1. IQ < 70. 2. Age < 6 years. 3. No psychosis. Operational Criteria: Comprehensive neurological, behavioral, and psycho- educational evaluation. Children were selected who met DSM-IZI criteria for ADD. Same sample as Rubenstein & Brown (1984). Lahey et al. (1985) 50 subjects (36 male, 14 female) Co-Occurring Diagnoses: Based on teacher ADD/H: n = 10 (9 male, 1 female) ratings on RBPC. Students had mild to se- ADD/WO: n = 20 (13 male, 7 female) vere behavior or social problems. Overlap- Normal controls: n = 20 (14 male, 6 fe- ping conduct disorders in 8/10 ADD/H male) subjects. Operational Criteria With RBPC: Behavioral Measures Results Durrell Reading Test Behavior Rating Scales: Abbreviated Con- ners (Teacher and Parent forms) Teacher Rating Scale of Attention (TRSA) Teacher Rating Scale of Impulsivity (TRSI) Yale Children's Inventory (Parent rating scale): 7 behavioral scales for ADD Attention Impulsivity Tractability Habituation Conduct Disorder Aggressive Negative Affect 3 cognitive scales Academic Language Fine Motor Matching Familiar Figures Test (MFFT; error score) WISC-R: Arithmetic Coding WRAT-Arithmetic Children's Checking Task (CCT) Conners Teacher Abbreviated Rating Scale Conners Parent Abbreviated Rating Scale Teacher Rating Scale of Attention Family Life Questionnaire Item analysis of 16 items of Attention Prob- lems-Immaturity scale of RBPC. Reason for Referral: 1. Poor schoolwork (females, 87%; males, 75%). 2. Behavior problems at school or home (M -- F). Conclusions: 1. ADD males and females have simi- lar profiles; however, ADD in females associated with more severe cognitive, language, and social defi- cits. 2. Factor analysis of Yale inventory yields: a. Attentional/Cognitive factor. b. Attention/Behavioral factor. Stepwise discriminant analysis yielded a sig- nificant discriminant function including 5 variables:WRAT-Arithmetic WISC-R: Arithmetic Coding Several Teacher Ratings of Attention Use of the resulting discriminant function resulted in correct classification of 70% of subjects. 1. ADD/H > ADD/WO: a. Irresponsibility. b. Distractibility. c. Impulsivity. d. Answering Without Thinking. e. Sloppiness. D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 Ackerman, Anhalt, 93 males Dykman, & Holcomb ADD/H: n = 24 (1986) ADD/WO: n = 21 Reading Disabled (RD): n = 24 Normal controls: n = 24 Ackerman, Anhalt, 93 males Holcomb, & Dykman ADD/H: n = 24 (1986) ADD/WO: n = 21 Reading disabled (RD): n = 24 Normal controls: n = 24 1. All ADD/H scored 2 1 standard deviation above mean (2 5) on Motor Excess factor. 2. No IQ criterion. 3. No report on status of medication. Co-Occurring Diagnoses: Experimental subjects referred for behavior problems or learning disabilities. Operational Criteria: 1. ADD groups defined on basis of Conners teacher ratings. 2. Exclusion of subjects whose VIQ and PIQ were both < 90. 3. Not on stimulant medication at time of ratings. Co-Occurring Diagnoses: Experimental subjects referred for behavlor problems or learning disabilities. Operational Criteria: 1. DSM-111 criteria employed in defi- nitions of ADD/H and A D D N O . 2. ADD groups defined on basis of Conners teacher ratings. 3. Exclusion of subjects whose VIQ and PIgwere bath < 90. 4. Not on stimulant medication at time of ratings. Semantic and acoustic encoding: 1. Free, acoustic. 2. Free, semantic. 3. Cued, acoustic. 4. Cued, semantic. List learning of high- and low-imag- ery words: 1. Learning, Hi. 2. Learning, Lo. 3. Delayed, Hi. 4. Delayed, Lo. Memory for 12 printed words: 1. Look. 2. Study. 3. Cluster. Computation: 1. Paper, correct. 2. Computer, correct. 3. Paper, latency. 4. Computer, latency. 1. Frequency-of-occurrence sensitivity. 2. Temporal sensitivity. 3. Speed of naming drawings of objects. 4. Speed of writing 0's and name. 5. Accuracy with number facts: a. Paper. b. Computer, nonreward. 6. Speed with number facts: a. Paper. b. Camputer. c. Computer with reward. 2. ADD/WO > ADD/H: a. Sluggishness. b. Slowness. ANCOVA results varying for IQ (for task with the corresponding number in previous column). Semantic and acoustic encoding: 1. No differences. 2. No differences. 3. RD < Control, ADD/H. 4. RD < ADD/WO. Hi-Lo imagery: 1. Control > ADD/WO. 2. Control > all. 3. No differences. 4. Control > all. Memory for words: 1. No differences. 2. Control > all. 3. No differences. Computation: 1. Control > all. 2. Control > all. 3. Control, RD < ADD/WO. 4. No differences. 1. Frequency sensitivity: No differences. 2. Naming (time): Young subjects-no dif- ferences; old subjects-ADD/H > Con- trol and RD. 3. Writing (time): Young subjects-no dif- ferences; old subjects-ADD/ WO sig- nifcantly less productive than ADD/H, RD, and Control groups. 4. Stepwise regression analysis exploring re- latian of data set to ADD Index con- cluded that ADD/WO and RD children do not have basic math hcts sutoms- tized; mean latencies in solving basic math facts yielded the strongest single predictor of the ADD/WO Index. (Continued) D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 Table 2. (Continued) Author(s) N Brown (1986) 146 subjects ADD/H: n = 88 (75 male, 13 female) ADD/WO: n = 58 (51 male, 7 female) Carlson et al. (1986) 51 subjects ADD/H: n = 20 ADD/WO: n = 15 Normal controls: n = 16 Conte et al. (1986) 24 subjects (18 male, 6 female) ADD/H: n = 8 (6 male, 2 female) ADD/WO: n = 8 (7 male, 1 female) Normal controls: n = 8 (5 male, 3 female) Differential Diagnosis Co-Occurring Diagnoses: All subjects referred to a university clinic for behavior problems including restlessness, distractibility, short attention span, and poor impulse control. Operational Criteria: 1. Onset of disorder prior to 7 years. 2. Differential diagnosis into ADD/H and ADD/WO groups based on in- dependent judgments of two raters. 3. No IQ criterion given but psycho- metric data provided. 4. Not on medication at time of ratings. Co-Occurring Diagnoses: All experimental subjects recruited from re- source programs for LD and BD students. Operational criteria: Groups defined on basis of SNAP by teacher: 1. ADD/H had ratings of pretty much or very much on: a. 2 2 hyperactivity symptoms. b. 2 3 inattention symptoms. c. 2 3 impulsivity symptoms. 2. ADD/WO had ratings of pretty much or very much on: a. < 2 hyperactivity symptoms. b. 2 3 inattention symptoms. c. 2 3 impulsivity symptoms. 3. WISC-R FSIQ > 80. Co-Occurring Diagnoses: Both experimental groups at least 2 years below grade level in reading, spelling, and math. All enrolled in learning-disabilities re- medial classes. Operational Criteria: 1. DSM-111 symptom checklists. 2. Revised Conners Teacher Rating Scale 3. WISC-R IQs > 85. 4. Not on medication at time of ratings. Behavioral Measures Results 10-item Abbreviated Conners Rating Scale 1. Teachers rated ADD/H children as completed by teachers. more problematic. 2. Three factors emerged: a. Emotional Lability/Conduct. b. Temperament. c. Attention-Concentration. WISC-R CELF VMI BASIS Rapid Naming Task Visual Match-to-Sample Task Detroit: Visual Attention Span Stroop Color Distraction WISC-R: 1. FSIQ: ADD/WO (98.87) > ADD/H (90.70). 2. VIQ: ADD/WO (99.64) > ADD/H (85.40). Timed tasks (Rapid Naming, Stroop): Controls faster than ADDs. Achievement: Controls > ADD/WO on math achievement. Visual Match to Sample: Controls > ADD/WO. ADD/H: No differences with controls or A D D N O . Matching Familiar Figures Test (MFFT) MFFT: Paired-Associate Learning Task (PA) 1. ADD/H latencies > ADD/WO la- tencies. 2. ADD/WO subjects more impulsive than ADD,% subjects. PA: - ADDs did not benefit from extra study time on fixed-rate lists. D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 Carlson et al. (1987) 72 subjects (predominantly male and Caucasian) ADD/H: n = 16 ADD/WO: n = 11 Normai controls: n = 45 Lahey et al. (1987) 63 subjects (predominantly male and Caucasian) ADD/H: n = 41 ADD/WO: n = 22 Lahey et al. (1988) 1. N = 667 nonreferred children. 2. N = 86 consecutive referrals to GCC. ADD/H: n = 41 ADD/WO: n = 22 Clinic Controls: n = 23 rommer, Hoeppner, 78 subjects 1 osenberg, Arm- ADD/H: n = 36 Etrong, & K~ths te~n 4DD/U!O: n = 12 (1988) Controls: n = 30 Exclusions: Co-occurring diagnoses except mild, non- disabling simple phobias excluded. Operational Criteria: 1. Children's version of Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophre- nia (K-SADS) conducted in inter- view format with teacher, parent, and child. 2. Rating scales were completed by parents and teachers. Co-Occurring Diagnoses: 1. ADD/H: 56% conduct disorder; 10% Internalizing disorder. 2. ADD/WO: 36% conduct disorder; 43% internalizing disorder (anxiety or depressive). Exclusions: 1. FSIQ < 70. 2. Neurological disorder. 3. Psychotic disorder. Operational Criteria: 1. Children's version of Schedule for Affective Disorder and Schizophre- nia (K-SADS) conducted in inter- view format with teacher, parent, and child. 2. Rating scales were completed by parents and teachers. Operational Criteria: 1. Children's version of Schedule for Affective Disorder and Schizophre- nia (K-SADS) conducted in inter- view format with teacher, parent, & child 2. Rating scales were completed by parents and teachers. Suhects were me&cally dentrfied No child was taklng stimulant or other medication mfluencing deep. Sociometric measures administered by class- room teachers. Children were asked to write down privately the names of 3 children liked most (LM), 3 childrenliked least (LL), and 3 who fought most. Behavioral Ratings: Conners Teacher Rating Scale (TRS) Child Behavior Rating Scale Children's Depression Inventory Trait Anxiety Scale SNAP RBPC Structured Interview: K-SADS 1. Teachers asked to rate children on SNAP. 2. Clinicians rated children on 20 descrip- tors of ADHD: 13 drawn from DSM-IIL 2 from DSM-111-R, and 5 items as- sociated with ADD/WO (sluggish, drowsy, absent-minded, daydreams, dis- turbs others). ParentalQyestjamire to determine the prevalence of sleep disorders at present (last 3 to 12 nonths> and in retrospect (last ! to 4 years). 1. Normals > ADD/H and A D D N O on Liked Most and Social Preference (i.e., LM - LL). 2. Normals < ADD/H and ADD/WO's on Least Liked. 3. No differences between ADD/H ADD/ WO. (However, when ADD/H ADD/ WO children with co-diagnoses were in- cluded in the analysis, ADD/H > ADD/WO and normals on Fights Most.) Teacher Ratings: 1. Conduct problems: ADD/H > ADD/WO. 2. Impulsivity: ADD/H > ADD/ WO. 3. Sluggish, cognitive tempo: ADD/ WO > ADD/H. 4. Fighting: ADD/H > ADD/WO. 5. ADD/WO more likely to display in- ternalizing disorders. 1. SNAP yielded two factors: a. Inattention/Disorganization. b. Motor Hyperactivity/Impulsivity. 2. Clinician-completed descriptors of ADD yielded three factors: a. Motor hyperactivity/Impulsivity. b. Inattention/Disorganization. c. Sluggish Tempo. ADDS Versus Controls: Significantly more parents of ADDS re- ported children having ti~db!e fa!!iiig asleep and feeling tired upon awakening. ADD/H Versus A D D N O : 1. ADD/H had greater nocturnal arousal than ADD/WO. 2. ADD/WO had greater morning re- call of nightmares than ADD/H. (Continued) D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 Table 2. (Continued) Author(s) N Differential Diagnosis Behavioral Measures Results - Barkley et al. (1990) 142 subjects Co-Occurring Diagnoses (%): Parental Source: Parental Interview: ADD/H: n = 42 (39 male, 3 female) Structured Interview 1. LD more likely to have experienced ADD/WO: n = 48 (43 male, 5 female) ADD/H ADD/WO LD Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale language delay. LD: n = 16 (12 male, 4 female) ADHD 92.9 31.3 0.0 Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) 2. ADD/H (40%) and A D D N O Normal controls: n = 36 (35 male, 1 fe- ODDa 40.5 18.8 0.0 Home Situations Questionnaire (HSQ) (54%) children have fair to poor male) CD a 21.4 6.3 0.0 Revised Conners Parent Rating Scale motor coordination significantly SAD a 9.5 6.3 0.0 Beck Depression Inventory more often than LD. OAD a 0.0 0.0 6.3 Symptom Checklist-Revised (SCL-9CR) 3. Significantly more ADD/H children MDD a 0.0 2.1 0.0 Locke-Wallace Marital Adjustment reported to have been treated with L D ~ R ~ ~ . 19.0 18.8 18.8 Life Stress scale from Parent Stress Index individual or family therapy and L D ~ s ~ ~ . 23.8 22.9 3 1.3 Teacher Source: placed in special education for be- ~ ~ ~ ~ a t h 26.2 20.8 3 1.3 Child Behavior Checklist-Teacher Report havior disorders. Form (CBCL-TRF) 4. Significantly more A D D N O than (aNote: ODD = oppositional defiant dis- School Situations Questionnaire ADD/H children in LD placements. order, CD = conduct disorder, SAD = sep- ADHD Rating Scale 5. ADD/H significantly more paternal aration anxiety disorder, OAD = overanx- Taxonomy of Problem Situations (TOPS) relatives with attention deficits and ious disorder, MDD = major depressive Psychological Tests: hyperactivity and maternal relatives disorder.) WISC-R with substance-abuse problems than (b~ote : LD defined as score below 7th %tile WRAT-R other groups. on achievement and significant discrepancy Continuous Performance Test (CPT) 6. Both ADD/WO and LD groups between IQ and achievement on that test.) Kagan Matching Familiar Figures Test more likely to have siblings with LD (MFFT) than ADD/H or normal groups. Parental Behavior Ratings: 1. ADD/H significantly worse than ADD/WO and other groups on number of problem settings (HSQ) and Aggressive and Delinquent scales (CBCL). Behavioral Observations: 2. ADD/WO significantly worse than nor- 1. ADHD behaviors during math prob- mals on Depressed, Uncommunicative, lems task (off-task, fidgets, out-of- Obsessive-Compulsive, Somatic Com- seat). plaints, Social Withdrawal, Aggressive, 2. ADHD behaviors during CPT test- and Delinquent scales of the CBCL. ing. Teacher Behavior Ratings: 3. Wrist and ankle actometers. 1. ADD/H worse than ADD/WO on pervasiveness, aggression, and self- destructive behaviors. 2. A D D N O worse than LDs or con- trols in 14 of 16 behavioral areas rated by teachers with exceptions in aggressiveness and problem situa- Operational Criteria: 1. For ADD/H: a. Maternal complaints of short at- tention span, impulsivity, and ove- ractivity at school. b. 6-month duration of problems. c. Age of onset: < 7 years. d. Child Attention Profile (CAP) scores > 93rd %tile on both Inat- tention and Overactivity scales. e. No history of stimulant treatment or removal of medication for 48 hr before evaluation. D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 Hynd, Lorys, et al. 20 subjects (1991) ADD/H: n = 10 (9 male, 1 female) ADD/WO: n = 10 (8 male, 2 female) Barkley et al. (1991) 40 subjects ADD/H: n = 23 (21 male, 2 female) ADD/WO: n = 17 (15 male, 2 female) 2. For ADD/WO: a. Same as criteria a, b, c, and e for ADD/H. b. CAP scores > 93rd Yotile on Inat- tention scale and < 84th %tile on Overactivity scale. 3. For LD: a. Referral for academic learning problems or current school LD placement. - - - - - - - -- - - - - - - - - - b. Teacher complaints of math, handwriting, or language. c. No teacher complaints of matten- tion, overactivity, or impulsivity. d. CAP scores < 84th %tile on both Inattention and Overactivity scales. Co-Occurring Diagnoses: 1. ADD/H: 40% conduct disordered. 2. ADD/WO: 60% developmental learning disorders. Excluslons: 1. FSIQ < 70. 2. Epilepsy. 3. Closed head injury. 4. Other neurological disorder. Operational Criteria: 1. WISGR: FSIQ > 70. 2. K-SADS: Parent and teacher. 3. Parent and teacher behavior rating scales (CBCL; SNAP). Co-Occurring Diagnoses: Not reported. Operational Criteria: Same as Barkley et al. (1990). Excluslons: SameeasBar&p etal.-(l99Dkplus the fol- low~ng further exclusions: 1. Childieii with history of tics oi T~i i - rette's. 2. Chlldren with history of cardiac sur- gery, high blood pressure, or cere- bral vascular accident. Academic Achievement: BASIS: Math, Reading, Spelling Behavioral Adjustment: SNAP: Hyperactivity, Inattention, and Im- pulsivity PIC: Social Skills Psychological Tests: 1. Verbal learning and memory: Wis- consin Selective Reminding Test (WSRT). 2. Vigilance: CPT. Zlmpulse e~ntml: KaganMEFT. 4. Behavior observations during math pioblems. Parent Behavior Ratings: 1. Home Situations Questionnaire (HSQj. 2. ADHD Rating Scale. tions with peers. ADD/WO children had no more peer relational diffi- culties than controls but were worse than LDs in this area. 3. ADD/H > ADD/WO on Inatten- tiveness scale of CBCL-TRF. 4. ADD/WO > ADD/H on Inatten- tiveness scale of CBCLTRF. Academic Achievement: BASIS Math: ADD/H > ADD/WO. Behavioral Adjustment: 1. SNAP: a. Hyperactivity: ADD/H > ADD/ WO. b. Inattention: No difference. c. Impulsivity: ADD/H > ADD/ wo. 2. PIC: ADD/H significantly more dis- turbed than ADD/WO based on parent ratings. Parent Behavior Ratings: 1. ADD/H worse than ADD/WO on number of problem settings on HSQ. 2. Main effect for drug condition in- dicating significant declines on: a. HSQ-Xumber of problem set- tings and mean severity scores with high doses (relztive to pla- cebo). b. ADHD Rating ScaleTota l score and number of significant symp- toms with all three doses (relative(Continued) D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 Table 2. (Continued) Author(s) N Differential Diagnosis Behavioral Measures Results 3. Children with history of adverse re- 3. Home Side Effects Rating Scale. actions to stimulants. Teacher Behavior Ratings: Operational Criteria: 1. School Situations Questionnaire Same as Barkley et al. (1990). (SSQ). 2. CAP. 3. Teacher Self-Control Rating Scale (TSCRS). 4. School Side Effects Rating Scale. to placebo, high doses, however, produced a significantly greater reduction than did low doses). Teacher Behavior Ratings: 1. ADD/H worse than ADD/WO on CAP, SSQ, and TSCRS. 2. Main effect for drug condition in- dicating significant declines on: a. CAP-Inattention with moderate and high doses. b. CAP-Overactivity with all three doses. c. SSQ-Number of problem set- tings and mean severity score at school reduced with all three doses. 3. Main effect for drug condition shows significant improvement in self-con- trol of both groups with all three doses. Note: ANCOVA = analysis of covariance; BASIS = Basic Academlc Skills Individual Screener; CCT = Children's Checking Task; CDI = Ch~ldren's Depression Inventory; FSIQ = Full Scale Intelligence Quotient; GCC = Georgia Children's Clinic; LD/BD = Learning Disabled/Behavior Disordered; PA = Picture Arrangement; PIC = Personality Inventory for Children; PIQ = Performance Intelligence Quotient; PPVT = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; RBPC = Revised Behavior Problem Checklist; VIQ = Verbal Intelligence Quotient; VMI = Test of Visual-Motor Integration; WISC-R = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised; WRAT = Wide Range Achievement Test. References for certain tests mentioned in table may be found in the relevant study. D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 SUBTYPES ( )F ADD cause IMaurer and Stewart's (1980) epidemiological study accounted for 16% of the total ADD/H sam- ple. A more realistic picture of the average sample size, then, is gained by excluding the Maurer and Stewart's (1980) retrospective case review. This re- sults irk an average sample size of 28.9 children with ADD/IH and 23.6 children with A D D N O per study. One: conclusion that can be drawn from these data, c~nsis~tent with that of Carlson (1986), is that relatively few children with ADDSWO have been studiecl. This problem is further compounded by recognition that the same subjects were used in sev- eral pairs of stuldies: Lahey et al. (1984) and Lahey et al. (1985); Lahey et al. (1987) and Lahey et al. (1988); Barkley et al. (1990) and Barkley, DuPaul, and NIcMurray (1991); and Ackerman, Anholt, Dykmjm, and FI01c0mb (1986) and Ackerman, An- holt, IIolcomb, and Dykman (1986). Further, re- view of the subject descriptions in Rubinstein and Brown (1984) and Brown (1986) suggests the possi- bility of sample overlap in these studies as well. If this is the case, the total number of ADDJWO chii- dren sl.udied may be as low as 395. Sex distribution. Subject populations in these studies havie been predominantly male, making up 91.5% of the t01,al sample. Five studies (Ackerman, Anhall., Dykman, & Holcomb, 1986; Ackerman, Anhalt, Holcomb, & Dykrnan, 1986; Dykman et al., 1980; Edelbrock et al., 1984; King & Young, 1982) have investigate~d exclusively male samples. On the other hand, five studies have included a much larger-thamaverage female sample (Berry, S. E. Shaywitz, 8c B. A. Shaywitz, 1985; Conte, Kins- bourne, Swansan, Zirk, & Sarnuels, 1986; Lahey et al., 19185; 1,ahey et al., 1984; Maurer & Stewart, 1980). Interestingly, the Maurer and Stewart (1980) and Conte et al. (1986) studies did not distinguish between ADD/H and ADD/WO groups whereas Lahey et a1 (1985), Lahey et al. (1984), and Berry et al. (1985) did, It is unknown how selection of an unusually large proportion of girls in these studies may hove aEectd outcomes. It is noteworthy that the Lahey et al. (1984,1985) subjects were experimentally classified using the Re- vised ELehavior Problem Checklist (Quay & Peter- son, 1983) rather than drawn from a clinic papula- tion. This may suggest that althou6h there are girls with ADD in the school setting, they are less likely to be referred than are boys with ADD. Such a conc1ua;ion agrees with Berry et al. ('985) who ex- press concern that there is a risk in underidentifying ADD girls without impulsivity. Chronological age. Ages of subjects participat- ing in tlhese behavioral studies range from 6.3 to 13.2 years. The average age is 9.4 years for ADD/H and 9.8 years for ADD/WO samples. 'The first conclu- sion that can be drawn from the ,age data is that a very narrow age range has beep studied. The second conchaion is that on tlhe average A D D N O chil- dren studied are appro:~imately M year older than ADD/H children. This tendency for ADD/WO subjects to be older thain ADD/H subjects suggests that A D D N O may be identified and/or referred later than ADDJH, as many of these subjects were from clinic-referred populatiops. This conclusion is consistent with Lahey et al. (1985')t who claim that children with ADD/H may b~ identified at younger ages because they are more ?otic:sable and create more distress for parents and teaclhers. Clinical versus nonl-eferre! samples. Samples for the behavioral studies emaQated from both clini- cal and nonreferred sources. ~ $ e vast majority of the studies were based on clinical +amples. On the other hand, several studies that ibitially diffferentiated ADDJH and ADDIWO groubs and proved heuris- tically valuable to this area of research were based largely on nonreferred subjectis. Lahey et al. (1984) and Lahey et al. (198S) exp#rime:ntally idientified ADD/H and A D D N O chil$ren from schwlwide teacher ratings of behavior. Tqis approach may well have included clinical subjects but was not limited to them. I Psychometric data. Psydborrietric data were collected on subjects in the majority of the behav- ioral studies (see Table 2). M p n IWechsler Intelli- gence Scale for Children-IRevised (WISC-R; Wechsler, 1974) scores were as follows: Verbal IQ (VIQ), 97.0 for ADDJH an 97.3 for ADD/WO childre& Performance 19 ( P h ) , 98.7 for ADD/H and 99.5 for ADD/WO clxildrkn; and Full Scale IQ (FSIQ), 102.01 for ADID/H 102.23 for ADD/ WO children. Only two et all., 1986; Dykman et al., 1980) ences between the In both cases, ADD/WO chpdren were found to have higher FSIQs than ADDqH children. The gen- eral conclusion that IQ clifFererjces between ADD/H and ADDIWO have not bedn significant (Hynd, Lorys, et al., 1991) is s ~ u ~ ~ o r t i d . Methodological Issues I Next, our discussion will focus on methodolo- gical issues related to behavioial studies of ADDS. Among the issues to be addressed are experimental design, diagnostic criteria for ADD/H and ADD/ WO, and the dependent measui-es employed in these studies. I D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 SUBTYPES OF ADD was solme variation, however, in how ADD/WO was defined (see Table 2). Dykman et al., for in- stance, required a parental interview to corroborate teacher ratings and developed additional ADD/WO requirements of dawdling or failure to complete classwork or homework not due to poor achieve- ment. 'Two important problems with this identifica- tion procedure are evident. First, impulsivity mea- sures may be neglected or confounded with attentional ratings. Second, use of both the Conners Teacher Rating Scales (Conners, 1969) and the Con- ners Abbreviated Teacher Rating Scales (Sprague et al., 19'74) to identify ADD children has been criti- cized as inappropriate by Ullman, Sleater, and Spraguie (1985)and Quay, Routh, and Shapiro (1987), This is an important issue considering use of the Conners scales in a fifth of the current ADD stud- ies ancl use as sole criterion in most of those. U11- mann et al. (1985) criticized use of both instru- ments. Regarding use of the 39-item rating scale, they point out substantive changes in 5 of the items in 1973 with no subsequent factor analysis from Conners. They also mention that analyses by other researchers have resulted in different factors from those first identified by Conners (e.g., Cohen & Hynd, 1986). Of particular concern for the mea- surement of ADD is the Werry, Sprague, and Coben (1975) factor analysis which yielded a com- bined I-Iyperactivity-Inattentiveness factor, suggest- ing that ADD/H and ADD/WO cannot be differ- entiated with this instrument. As B. A. Shaywitz (1987) claimed, the Conners scales yield diagnosis of heteroge~ieous samples of inattentive, overactive, and ag,gressive children. Regarding the Abbreviated Conners Scale, U11- mann let al. (1985) indicated that only 2 of the 10 items concern attention. Furthermore, they reported that wlhen using the widely accepted cutoff score of 15 for ithis s~cale, children with ADDJWO cannot be identified. To complicate this problem further, they have recommended increasing the cutoff score based on recent data, a change which most likely would increase the inappropriateness of this scale for diag- nosis of AI)D/WO. Brown (11986) also addressed concerns regarding measmement with the Abbreviated Conners. He cautioned against reliance on total scores, conclud- ing that the same total score on this instrument can have different implications for different ADD chil- dren. It is possible that qualitative differences in attention may exist but be obscured by total score interpretations as Barkley et al. (1990) have found witb the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1983). Clearly, there seem to be signifi- cant psychometric concerns regarding the use of the Conners scales in the diagnosis of ADD. A second approach to ADD subgrouping is evi- dent in the King and Young (19812) and Carlson et al. (1986) studies. Both utilize the SNAP (Pelham, Atkins, & Murphy, 1981) checklist, a listing of DSM-111 symptoms, to define AC)ID/H and ADD/ WO groups. These researchers use~d identical crite- ria for ADD/H identilicatioa. Again, as with the Conners scales, researchers vary in the criteria they employ even when using the sarne instrument to identify ADDMO. The two proups of researchers were consistent in their definitions lolf inattention and impulsivity; however, Icing apd Young (1982) de- fined the absence of hyperactivity more strictly than did Carlson et al. (1986; see Table 2). Although some variation im the ADD/WO defi- nition remains with the SNAP, one problem en- countered by the first two ADD subgrouping ap- proaches is considered. Quay let al (1987), in their review of measurement of phildhood psychopa- thology, point out that none af the existing teacher or parent rating scales except the SNAP have a sepa- rate impulsivity subscale. The implication is that none of the rating scales excep$ the SNAP will iden- tify specifically subjects meeting all1 three compo- nents of DSM-111 criteria (i.e., cleficient attention and impulsivity for ADD/WO and ADD/H with the additional requirement of Ihyperactivity for ADDJH). This idea has been challlenged, however, by the Lahey et al. (1988) resylts. Factor analysis of the SNAP yielded support foronl!? two factors, one of which was an overlapping voter Hypteractivity/ Impulsivity factor. A third approach to ADD yubgrouping is seen in the research of Lahey et al. (1984, 1985). These investigators defined their ABD s~xbgroups experi- mentally using the Revised Behavior Problems Checklist (RBPG; Quay & Peterson, 1983). As men- tioned with several other appqoaches to ADD sub- grouping, the RBPC mary not i$clude an impulsivity measure. If it does, it overlaps with other measures such as attention or motor excess which could result in heterogeneous ADD ,groupi gs. Therefore, its use as a sole criterion for AD11 di 1 etren~tiation yields an incomplete or obscured meashrern~snt of the ADD criteria as delineated in DSML-111: The fourth approach ito q D I ) subgrouping is found in Lahey et al. (1987); Carlson et al. (1987); Hynd, Lorys, et al. (1991); and Lithey et al. (1988) for their referred samples. Parqnts itad teachers were interviewed using structured interviews and both completed the SNAP and additionla1 behavior rat- ings. A11 diagnostic decisions were: made indepen- dently by two psy~h01o~:ists afiter reviewing all psy- chometric and behavioral data.. All diagnostic criteria reflected criteria outlived in DSM-111 for ADD/H and ADDJWO. Any disagreements were resolved through mutual discussior~. Cohen's kappas D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 were computed to determine reliability of this proce- dure (Spitzer, Cohen, Fleiss, & Endicott, 1967). This approach represents an improvement over previous approaches in that it employs multimodal behav- ioral assessment. Additionally, this approach gives attention to the issue of reliability of clinical judg- ments. On the other hand, some of the behavior rating scales utilized may yield heterogeneous ADD groupings. The fifth approach to ADD subtyping is that of Barkley et al. (1990, 1991). They were the first to employ an empirically derived approach to identify their subjects. They utilized the Child Attention Profile (CAP; see Barkley et al., 1991) and required similarly high scores on Inattention but discrepant scores on Overactivity to distinguish ADD/H and ADD/WO subjects. This approach appears to be the most objective presently available. However, it relies primarily on information from teachers, which may violate the accepted norm of multimodal be- havioral assessment. The final approach to ADD subgrouping used in the behavioral studies relies on clinical judgment. There were three studies (Brown, 1986; Edelbrock et al., 1984; Rubinstein & Brown, 1984) in which the clinical judgment of two independent raters using DSM-III criteria was the criterion for inclusion in the study; a fourth study (Berry et al., 1985) also used DSM-I11 criteria whereas a fifth (Maurer & Stewart, 1980) did so only partially. In the Edel- brock et al. (1484) study, two child psychiatrists reviewed case histories independently and assigned subjects to #DD/H and A D D N O subgroups. Rubinstein and Brown (1984) and Brown (1986) based their ADD subgroupings on the independent judgments of two clinic staff members who observed and rated each subject's behavior during interaction with a staff member in a "structured, classroom-like, task-oriented settling'"Brown, 1986, p. 97). In Berry et al. (1985), all subjects received comprehensive neurological, behaviord, and psychoeducational evaluation that inoluded patent and teacher rating scales, Howevor, db diagnostic reliabilities were re- ported for DSM-iII diagnoses. Two immediate concerns with these identibation procedures are ap- parent. First, reliability of diagnoses is a major issue. Second, the effeats of a one-to-one observational set- ting on attentiori are of concern. Regarding rdiab$lity, Edelbrock et al. (1984) re- ported consensus diagnosis of their subjects. Lahey et al. (1987); Lahq et al, (1988); and Hynd, Lorys, et al, (1991) veported adequate reliabilities for their ADD diagna~e5 bssed on the recommended kappa criterion of .7P ('spitzer et al., 1967). Rubinstein and Brown (1984) failhd to meet this reliability criterion for both AJYDdH (.37) and ADD/WO (.50) sub- jects. Brown (1986) demonstrated adequate reliabil- ity for ADD/H (.73) using this criterion but not for ADD/WO diagnoses (.62). Regarding the use of a one-to-one observationin the clinic setting (i.e., subjects with a staff member) for the purpose of diagnosis, Ross and Ross (1982) and Denckla and Heilman (1979) have pointed out the variability in the behavior of ADD subjects. They have suggested that when in a one-to-one set- ting such as a clinic office, the ADD children's be- havior might be quite appropriate in marked con- trast to typical classroom behavior. Such findings argue against the appropriateness of the diagnostic rating procedures utilized in the Rubenstein and Brown (1984) and Brown (1986) studies. Several conclusions can be drawn regarding the operationalization of ADD subtypes. First, there is a lack of agreement regarding how to identify ADD subtypes. This observation is consistent with that of B. A. Shaywitz (1987). Second, many of the studies have utilized unidimensional ide~tification proce- dures, many of which have increased risk of yielding heterogeneous ADD groups. Third, as noted by Meents (1989), diagnosis is presently rating-scale driven. Use of such diagnostic procedures does not allow, however, for documentation of each ADD symptom through rating-scale usage. Fourth, many of the rating scales used wme standardized prior to the 1980 subtyping of ADD in DSM-III. This sug- gests the posisibility that these measures may have attention scales that also contain characteristics of hyperactivity, thus yielding hatterogeneaus ADD groups, Fifth, almost 20% of the reported studies used clinical jpdgment withoat acceptable levels of diagnostic reliability reported or without any reli- ability check (Barkley et al., 1990). Finally, it should be noted that there is one DSM-III critedon for ADD that has been ignored by all but 4 of the 21 studies. DSM-III criteria require that ADD prob- lems have an age of onset by 7 ye@rs. Only Dykman et al. (1980), B r m (1986), and Bat'kley et al. (1990, 1991) reported having included this additi~naj orite- rion in their research. Differentiation of ADD Subtypes The final methodological issue to consider is which behavioral measures have been employed to distinguish ADD/H and ADDlWO. The behav- ioral studies have utilized 37 different instruments in conjunction with DSM-III criteria to assess possi- ble differences between ADD/H and ADD/WO. These measures fall into the following categories: behavior rating scales, structured interviews, soci- ometric rathgs, self-report measures of self-concept and depression, intelligence measures, achievement tests, lanpage tests, perceptual-motor tests, mem- ory tests, tests of presumed innate and acquired au- D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 SUBTYPES OF ADD tomatic pracesses, measures of sustained attention, and tests of impulsivity. Among these, it can be concluded that the mea- sures that have differentiated the ADD subgroups successfully include behavior rating scales, sociom- etric ratings, self-report measures, math ability/ achievement measures, long-term memory mea- sures, and presumed measures of acquired automatic processes (Ackerman, Anhalt, Holcomb, & Dykman, 1986). In contrast to the conclusion of Carlson et al. (1986) "that the difference between the two groups may be more behavioral and emotional than c~ognitive" (p. 84), differences in both behav- ioral and cognitive areas are documented. Barkley et al. (1990) pointed out that some of the behaviloral diffe~rentiations are clouded by possible confounding of independent and dependent varia- bles in several studies that relied on teacher behav- ior-rating scales (Lahey et al., 1985, 1987). They acknowledge that this is also a partial problem (lim- ited to certain behavior ratings as dependent varia- bles) for their results. Partial confounding limited to behavilor ratings is also noted in Hynd, Lorys, et al. (1 99 1) Behavi~oral Distinctiveness of ADD/W and ADD/WO Con~sideration needs to be given to the distinctive clinical presentation of ADD/H and A D D N O children that has resulted from the behavioral stud- ies. Although caution has been urged in interpreting behavi~oral findings, a review reveals that though most behavioral distinctions were established origi- nally with experimentally classified subjects (Lahey et al., 1984, 1985) and averlapping samples, these behaviloral differences now have been confirmed using clinical subjects and independent samples (Barkley et al., 1990; Berry et al., 1985; Hynd, Lorys, et al., 1991; Lahey et al., 1987). Results of the behavioral studies then demonstrate possible differ- ences between the ADD/H and ADD/WO children in beha~vior, emotions, social interaction, academics, family background, and response to intervention. The resulting clinical profiles of the two groups are quite different. Considering the three primary features of ADD outlined in DSM-III, consistent with D'SM--III criteria for ADD/H, this group is empirically more active than the ADD/WO group but haa levells of inattentiveness similar to those for ADD/WO. Importantly, however, qualitative dif- ferences in inattention have been reported between the twcrl groups by Barkley et al. (1990). On the other hand, lincon~sistent with DSM-111 criteria, ADD/ WO children have been documented empirically to be less impulsive than ADD/H children (Berry et al., 1985; H[ynd, Lorys, et al., 1991; Lahey et al., 1985, 1987, 1988; Maurer & Stewart, 1980) and not more impulsive than normal co~ntrols (King & Young, 1982). Findings, of Conte t:t al. (1985) and Barkley et al. (1990) dispute this c~onclusion by sug- gesting similar levels of impulsi~rity in the two groups. However, probliems with these two studies related to this particuliar point sl?ould be noted. First, Conte et al. (1985) is an butliex on three of the f ~ u r subject variables reviewed, having a smaller sample (8 subjects per group), higher percentage of girls, and among the oldest sudjects studied. Second, Barkley et al. (1990) reported l y i n g a high percent- age of overlapping diagnoses f oppositional defiant disordler among their AlDD d 0 group, which most likely influenced findings regarding impulsivity. It could be inferred, then, from dhese: findings that the UADD criteria of DSA4-IIIJ-R lmay be: more ap- propriate for identification of ADlD/WO because impulsivity is omitted from the di:tgnostic criteria. This conclusion is consistent wi1,h Lahey et al. (1988). Following some preliminary work with experi- mentally classified ADD saupbs, Lahey et al. (1985) suggested that A D D / ~ O rnay classify along the Internalizing dimension ofl behavior rather than the Externalizing dirnensiod as does ADD/H (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 198'3). It now appears on the basis of additional clinical(~ark1e~ et al., 1990) and factor-analytic data (Berrx et al., 1985; Lahey et d., 1988) that more credence slhoulti be given to this hypothesis. In this regardc B$rry let al. found evi- dence of separate ~ttentionalRCo~riitive ancl Atten- tionalJBehaviora1 factors on the Yale Children's In- ventory (S. E. Shaywitz, Schn$ll, B. A. Shaywitz, & Towle, 1986). Further, Lahe$ et id. (1988) found support for a Motor Hyperact vity/'lmpulsivity fac- i tor, an Inattention/Dinorgan$zatica factor, and a Sluggish Tempo factor on the SNAP. Tbe possibility that ADD/WO is a rpore ;ilctentional/cognitive/ anxiety type of disordta in yontnsst to the more attentional/behavioral/impul#vs aspects of ADD./H must be consitlered. In fa& Hynd, Lorys, et al. (1991) and Barkley et dl. (1990) have postu- lated that the two groups ma$ represent diqsimilar psychiatric disorders. In recent work, Barlkley eb al. (1990) described both ADD groups as nnderqchieving, having im- paired learning, and having qroblems with incom- plete work. It is possible, if nc/t likely, based on the present clinical profiles that qu litative differences in the two groups underpin simi 1 ar levelsof impaired school performance just as qualit,ative differences are documented in inattentiom As Hynd, Lorys, et al. (1991) suggested, poor school performance among ADD/H chi1dr1:n may be linked to atten- tionalhehavioral problems whereas it appears that poor school performance amon,g children with D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 A D D N O may be linked to more attentional/cog- nitive disabilities. This idea is supported by recent documentation of links between ADD/WO and learning disabilities (Hynd, Lorys, et al., 1991). To conclude, the review of behavioral research seems to suggest some significant behavioral differ- ences between ADD/H and ADD/WO. However, the serious clinical and methodological issues raised by this review, such as small total number of ADD/ WO children studied, widely varying operational definitions of ADD/WO, incomplete appraisal of DSM-111 criteria, presence of overlapping dis- orders, confounding of independent and dependent measures, and lack of controlled studies in some cases, necessitate considering these behavioral dif- ferences as tentative evidence of the empirical valid- ity of the ADD/WO classification. Although some evidence for cognitive differences between ADD/H and ADD/WO exists, few of these differences have emerged through the use of traditional psychometric data in these behavioral studies (Hynd & Willis, 1988). To investigate fur- ther the merit of distinct categories of attention def- icit disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 1980, 1987), attempts have been made to distinguish ADD/H and ADD/WO on neurocognitive mea- sures. Neuropsychological studies may shed light on relatively unique neurocognitive patterns of abilities or deficits associated with subtypes of ADD. Fur- ther, results from these studies may suggest which neurological systems or structures may be impaired in ADD. Weuropsychological Studies of ADD Presently, there are 10 studies that have at- tempted to differentiate ADD/H from ADD/WO on the basis of neuropsychological measures; these are summarized in Table 3. At a conceptual level, these studies attempt to link disordered behavior to neurological/neuropsychological systems that may be dysfunctional. Because the focus of these studies is etiological, they are discussed separately from the more behavioral studies which should be considered more descriptive. This section addresses clinical 'and methodological issues associated with these neuro- psychological studies. Subject Characteristics Sample size. Neuropsychological studies have contrasted 256 ADD/H subjects with 177 ADD/ WO subjects, 125 clinic controls, and 97 normal controls. These total figures, however, are mislead- ing because 91 of the 93 subjects in Holcomb, Acker- man, and Dykman (1985) also participated as sub- jects in Holcomb, Ackerman, and Dykman (1986) as did 20 of the Hynd, Lory's et al. (1991) subjects in the Goodyear (1990) study. Further, of the re- maining subjects, all but 139 were evaluated through the same university clinic, so the possibility exists that subject populations overlapped, further reduc- ing the total number of subjects (Hynd et al., 1989; Schaughency, Lahey, Hynd, Stone, & Piacentini, 1990; Stone, 1986). If there were maximal overlap of the samples in that clinic, then the total number of different subjects included in the neuropsychological studies would be reduced to 121 ADD/H subjects, 88 ADD/WO subjects, 40 clinic controls, and 74 normal controls, which are small numbers for draw- ing any firm conclusions about neuropsychological differences of ADD/H and ADD/WO children. Average sample size for these studies was 25.6 for ADD/H and 18.7 for ADD/WO, somewhat smaller than in the behavioral studies. Sex distribution. As with the behavioral stud- ies, the subject samples in the neuropsychological studies have been predominantly male (87% of the total reported sample). Both Holcomb et al. (1985, 1986) studies as well as Sergeant and Scholten (1985) investigated only boys. On the other hand, the Hynd et al. (1989) study had a larger proportion of girls than the other neuropsychological studies. It is unknown how a higher proportion of girls might affect neuropsychological outcomes, but Berry et al. (1985) found behavioral differences among ADD subtypes related to sex. Trommer, Hoeppner, Lorber, and Armstrong (1 988) did not report the sex of their subjects. Chronological age. Ages of subjects participat- ing in the neuropsychological studies ranged from 8.6 to 12.9 years. The average age was 9.5 years for children with ADD/H and 10.8 years for children with ADD/WO. ADD/WO subjects in the neuro- psychological studies were a year older on average than A D D m O subjects in behavioral studies (i.e., 10.8 and 9.8 years, respectively). Psychometric data. Psychometric data were collected on all subjects in the neuropsychological studies with the exception of those investigated by Sergeant and Scholten (1985) and Trommer, Ho- eppner, Lorber, and Armstrong (1988). Mean WISC-R scores were as follows: VIQ, 103.7 for ADD/H and 101.5 for ADD/WO children; PIQ, 102.0 for ADD/H and 99.5 for ADD/WO children; and FSIQ, 102.4 for ADD/H and 102,l for ADD/ WO children, Mean IQ scores from behavioral and neuropsychological resaarch are very consistent, ex- cept for VIQ for both ADD groups, which was higher in the neuropsychological studies. In none of these studies did the FSIQs of ADD/WO subjects D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 SUBTYPES OF ADD signific:antllr exceed those of ADD/H subjects. Con- sidering psjrchometric data available from both be- havior,al and neuropsychological studies, 10 of 12 studies, show no differences in FSIQs for ADD/WO and ADD/H groups. Therefore, the Dykman et al. (1980) and Carlson et al. (1986) findings where FSIQ differences were documented (ADD/WO > ADD/'H) must be considered unusual. Metho~dological Issues Discussion now turns to methodological issues related to r~europsychological studies of ADDS. Is- sues related to experimental design, diagnostic crite- ria, and dependent measures are examined. Com- parisoins between neuropsychological and behavioral studies are presented. Experimental Design In contrast to the behavioral studies, all but two of the neuropsychological studies employed experi- mental designs comparing ADD/H, ADD/WO, and co~ntrol groups (Good year, 1990; Hynd, Lorys, et al., 1991). Most of the controlled studies used clinical subjects as controls (see Table 3 for a de- scription of these clinical controls). Three of the eight (Holcomb et al., 1985, 1986; Stone, 1986) stud- ies included both clinical and normal controls. Based on tliese findings, experimental design of the neuropsych~ological research appears adequate. Differential Diagnosis of ADD As seen in the review of behavioral studies, there are t w ~ ~ differential diagnostic issues to consider. For the nt:uropsychological studies both co-occurring diagnoses and operational criteria for defining ADDlH and ADD/WO groups are addressed. Co-occurring diagnoses. Similar to the findings with tlhe behavioral studies, the possibility that sub- jects have overlapping academic and/or behavioral problems exists in all but one of the neuropsycholog- ical studies, as most samples were drawn from re- ferred popidations. Reflecting their concern about this issue, Stone (1986); Hynd et al. (1989); and Hynd, Lorys, et al. (1991) provided more specific data albout co-occurring diagnoses of their subjects. Across these three studies, the higher percentage of phobias among their ADD/WO as compared to ADDl'H subjects might have been predicted be- cause lLahey et al. (1985) concluded on the basis of behavioral data that ADD/WO may be an internal- izing disorder.However, the co-occurrence of con- duct disorders in more than one third of the ADD/ WO subjects seems much higher than what would have been expected with an internalizing disorder. Such a finding casts doubt on the appropriateness of the operationalization of ADD/W(C) in these studies. This is a serious issue because the same operational criteria used by Hynd et al. (1989) were also em- ployed by Schaughency et al. (1990) and Stone (1986) and because no significant differences be- tween ADD/H and ADD/WO giroups were found in any of these studies. On the other hand, employing a different clinic sample Hynd, Lorys, et al. $1991) reported more predictable co-occurrinlg diaqnoses (Lahey et al., 1985). Interestingly, 601% of lhe .PtDD/WO group had co-occurring deve11opmeqtalle:arning disorders whereas none of the AI)D/H Subj~ects did. It is rele- vant to note that significant repultr; were obtained in this study where co-occurring diagnoses were in the predicted direction. Operational criteria~. The second methodolo- gical issue to be addressed re ardirlg differential di- agnosis of ADD/H and KD /W(3 concerns what 4 operational criteria were used t o define the two sub- groups. Intellectual, medical, dnd behavioral criteria are considered. Exclusionary IQ criteria w$re included in the op- erational criteria of 8 of the 10 neuropsychological studies. Exclusion of subjects from these studies was based on criteria ranging fro9 IC! < 90 (Holcomb et al., 1985, 1986) to IQ < to 69 (Hynd et al., 1989; Schaughency et <al., Stone, 1986). Ser- geant and Scholten (19851) did not utilize IQ - exclusionary criteria, reasone that they had a group with normal IQ because all s bjects were in regular education classes. This does ot, however, rule out icaps in their sample. I the possibility of mild cognitipe or behavioral hand- , Operational criteria regariing medication were provided in 3 of the 10 neuro sycliological studies. P Both Holcomb et al. (1985, 4986) studies required that subjects be free of mledicbtion at least 2 weeks prior to dependent measur . Holwever, status of medication during the dliagno 7 tic phase was omitted. In the remaining studie:~, no qepont on the status of medication was provided. Review of operational crit&ria For the behavioral definitions of ADD/H and A D/IiVO in the neuro- psychological studies reveal four different ap- t proaches to ADD subgroupiqg. First, as alluded to earlier, the Stone (1986); Schdughency et al. (1990); Hynd et al. (1989); and Lor s, Hlynd, and Lahey i. (1990) studies employed the same operational crite- ria. They utilized multidimenqional assessment pro- cedures which included inforbation from multiple sourcles across multiple situatibns. Group formation was based on interviews, obdervai.ions, and rating scales. To arrive at inldepenflent DSM-III diag- noses, the diagnosticiar~s reviawedl results of the in- D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 Table 3. Neuropsychological Studies of Children Diagnosed as Having ADD/H or ADD/ WO Neuropsychological Author($ N Differential Diagnosis Measures Results Holcomb et al. (1985) 93 males Operational Criteria: Visual ERPs were measured while subjects 1. WISC-R Controls > ADDS. ADD/H: n = 24 1. All subjects required to have FSIQ were engaged in RT task to a low-probabil- 2. No significant differences among the four ADD/WO: n = 21 > 90. ity nonsense trigram and either low-proba- groups in N2, Nc, or SW amplitude. RD: n = 24 2. All subjects free of stimulants for at bility symbols or 3-letter words. 3. P3 amplitude: Controls significantly > Normal controls: N = 24 least 2 weeks prior to ERP testing. 1. Amplitudes of ERP components N2, clinical groups. Trend: ADD/H > RD 3. ADD/H criteria: P3, SW, Nc, and PC were measured. > ADD/WO. a. Raw score > 15 on teacher-rated 2. Latency of P3 component was mea- 4. Difference between target and nontarget Abbreviated Conners including sured. P3 amplitude: Controls > ADDS. adverse rating (2 or 3) on the three 5. Target P3 amplitude: Controls > ADD/ core motoric items. Mean rating WO. = 20.2. 6. Latency P' component to target and non- b. WRAT Reading and Spelling > target stimuli: Controls < ADDs. 90. 7. Latency target P's in Block 2 compared c. Clinical rating of ADD/H by psy- to Block 1: ADD/H and ADD/WO > chiatric evaluation team. Controls. 4. A D D 1 7 0 criteria: 8. P3 latency to unexpected stimuli: Con- a. Low ratings (1 or 0) on three core trols < ADDS. motoric items of Conners. Mean rating = 10.9. b. WRAT Reading and Spelling > 90. c. Clinical rating of ADD/WO by psychiatric evaluation team using DSM-ZZZ criteria. 5. RD and Control criteria: Rating < 15 on Teacher Abbreviated Conners. Mean RD rating = 10.3; mean nor- Sergeant & Scholten 24 males (1985) ADD/H: n = 8 ADD/WO: n = 8 Normal controls: n = 8 Holcomb et al. (1986) 91 males ADD/H: n = 23 ADD/WO: n = 21 mal-control rating = 2.4. Co-Occurring Diagnoses: All children attended "normal" school. Operational Criteria: All subjects received both observer and High-speed visual search task. teacher behavior ratings. Parent interviews. Developmental neurological examination. 1. All ADD/H children significantly slower and less accurate than controls not due to slower performance in stages of encod- ing, search, and/or decision. Therefore, no deficit of selective attention, only slower cognitive processing. 2. ADD/WO children significantly slower search rate than controls. This suggests possible selective-attention deficit. Operational Criteria: Auditory ERPs were measured while sub- 1. WISC-R: Controls > ADDS. 1. All subjects required to have FSIQ jects were engaged in RT task to a low-prob- 2. RT of controls significantly faster than > 90. ability auditory stimulus. clinical groups. Clinical groups did not D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 RD: n = 24 Normal controls: n = 23 Stone (1986) 76 subjects ADD/H: n = 29 (86% male) A D D N O : n = 18 (89% male) Clinic controls: n = 19 (63% male) Normal controls: n = 10 (80% male) Trommer, Hoeppner, 76 subjects Lorber, & Armstrong ADD/H: n = 28 (1988) A D D N O : n = 16 Normal Controls: n = 32 2. All subjects free of stimulant for at least 2 weeks prior to ERP testing. 3. ADD/H criteria: a. Raw score > 15 on teacher-rated Abbreviated Conners including adverse rating (2 or 3) on the three core motoric items. Mean rating = 20.2. b. WRAT Reading and Spelling > 90. c. Clinical rating of ADD/H by psy- chiatric evaluation team. 4. ADD/WO criteria: a. Low ratings (1 or 0) on three core motoric items of Conners. Mean rating = 10.9. b. WRAT Reading and Spelling > 90. c. Clinical rating of ADD/WO by psychiatric evaluation team using DSM-I11 criteria. 5. RD and Control criteria: Rating < 15 on Teacher Ab- breviated Conners. Mean RD rating = 10.3; mean normal-control rating = 2.4. Co-Occurring Diagnoses: Referrals from mental health clinic, juvenile services, schools, and physicians. Exclusion of children with FSIQ < 65 or psychotic behavior. ADD/H: 66% co-occurring diag- noses. ADD/WO: 44% co-occurring diag- noses. Operational Criteria: Same as Schaughencv et ai. (1990). Cohen's kappas: kDb/Ei: .77 ADD/WO: .80 ~ o ~ ~ c c u r r i n ~ ~ i a ~ ~ o s e s : Both ADD groups heterogeneous for learn- ing disabilities judged based on special edu- cation or tutoring. Parent Interview on DSM-III criteria, Conners Parent Que3- tionnaire, No medication at time of testing. 1. Amplitudes of ERP components N1, P2, N2, and P~~ were measured. 2. Latency of P3b component was ex- amined. Children's Checking Task, a measure of sus- tained attention in which subjects listened to audiotape of numbers and had a five-page booklet with rows of numbers. Subjects crossed out numbersthat matched tape and circled errors. Errors of omission and com- mission were tabulated. - - - - ~ u d i o t a ~ e d of go-no-go para- digm, 5 go signals (single tap); 5 nn-go sig- nals (two taps) in fixed random sequence at 3-sec intervals. Child should raise and lower index finger to "go" srgnal, but ignore "no- go." dlffer significantly from one another. Trend: RD > ADD/H > A D D N O . 3. P2 amplitude to unexpected stimuli: RD and ADD/H > ADD/WO and Con- trols. 4. No significant dlfferences among the 4 groups on N1, N2, and P3a amplitudes. 5. Controls had greater ~3 bS on target trials than clinical groups. 6. SW: Controls significantly > ADDS. 7. PC: Control > R D and ADD/WO. No significant group differences for either omission or commission errors. 1. c o n t r o l < ADD/H or ADD/WO in total errors 2. A D D N O > Control or ADD/H in total errors and commission errors on first trial. 3. ADD/WO greater improvement than ADD/H on second trial. (Continued) D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 Table 3. (Continued) Neuropsychological Author($ N Differential Diagnosis Measures Results Hynd et al. (1989) 81 subjects ADD/H: n = 43 (81% male) ADD/WO: n = 22 (77% male) Clinic controls: n = 16 (78% male) Goodyear (1990) 39 subjects ADD/H: n = 18 A D D N O : n = 21 Lorys et al. (1990) 100 subjects (79 male, 21 female; 87 Cauca- sian, 12 African-American, 1 Hispanic) ADD/H: n = 48 A D D N O : n = 26 Clinic control: n = 26 Co-Occurring Diagnoses: Referred population. ADD/H: 56% conduct disordered, 9.3% phobias. ADD/WO: 36% conduct disordered, 27% phobias. Exclusion of Children With: 1. FSIQ < 69. 2. Neurological disorder. 3. Psychotic disorder. Operational Criteria: 1. Children's version of Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophre- nia (K-SADS) conducted in inter- view format with teacher, parent, and child. 2. Rating scales were completed by parents and teachers. Same as Hynd et al. (1989). RT Measures: 1. Simple RT to visual stimulus (i.e., light). 2. Physical Match-Letter Pairs: RT to decide if two letters were physical match. 3. Name Match-Letter Pairs: RT to decide if two letters were semanti- cally the same. 4. Letter-String Match: RT to decide if two nonsense trigrams were the same. Boston Naming Test Woodcock Reading Mastery Test-Revised Co-Occurring Diagnoses: WISC-R: Both ADD groups contained children with 1. Verbal Comprehension. sole diagnoses of ADD/H and ADD/WO 2. Perceptual Organization. as well as children with conduct disorder as 3. Freedom From Distractibility. a secondary diagnosis. 4. Digit Span. - 4. ADD/H > Control in errors on second t rU. 5. ADD/H equally impulsive (number of commission errors) both trials. 6. Greatest frequency of errors in youngest subjects. 1. No significant differences between ADD/H and ADD/WO groups. 2. ADD/H did not perform as well as Con- trol on letter-string match. 1. ADD/WO nearly equal to ADD/H on Boston total scores; however, ADD/WO > ADD/H in number of cues needed. 2. Word attack > word recognition for ADD/WO but not ADD/H. WISC-R: No significant results. LNNB-CR: 1. Item 128: No significant differences. 2. Item 65: No significant differences. D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 Exclusion of Children With: 1. FSIQ < 70. 2. Psychotic disorders. 3. Neurological disorders. 4. Conduct disorders excluded from clinic controls. Operational Criteria: Same as Schaughency et al. (1990). Schaughency et al. 55 subjects (predominantly male) Co-Occurring Diagnoses: (1990) ADD/H: n = 25 Referral sources were parents, physicians, ADD/WO: n = 14 schools, and state center for severe emo- Clinic controls: n = 16 tional and behavior disorder. Exclusion of Children With: 1. FSIQ < 70. 2. Psychotic behavior. 3. Neurological disorder. 4. Conduct disorders from clinic con- LNNB-CR: 1. Memory scale: Item 128 (serial learning). 2. Visual scale: Item 65 (visual-spatiai orientation). 3. Rhythm scale: Items 41, 42. 4. Motor scale: Items 16, 17. RT: Task required 3-letter string match. Sensory Localization: 1. Left finger agnosia. 2. Right finger agnosia. LNNB-CR 3. Items 41, 42: ANCOVA with chronological age (CA) as only covariate. ADD/H and climc con- trol group differed @ < .U5). 4. Items 16, 17: Post hoc comparisons between ADD/H and clinic control were significant (p < .036). Sensory Localization: Post hoc tests yielded significant results between ADD/H and ADD/WO. No significant differences on any of the clini- cal or special scales (i.e., Motor, Rhythm, Tactile, Visual, Receptive and Expressive Speech, Writing, Reading, Arithmetic, Memory, and Intelligence). trols. Operational Criteria: 1. Children's version of Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophre- nia (K-SADS) conducted in inter- view format with teacher, parent, and child. 2. Rating scales were completed by parents and teachers. Hynd, Lorys, et al. 20 subjects (predominantly Caucasian; Co-Occurring Diagnoses: Rapid Naming: Rapid Naming: (1991) lower-middle to middle-class SES; no psy- 1. ADD/H: 40% conduct disordered. 1. Color Naming (RAN). ADD/WO slower than ADD/H on RAN/ chiatric hospitalizations) 2. ADD/WO: 60% developmental 2. Color, Number, and Letter Naming RAS Combined Naming. ADD/H: n = 10 (9 male, 1 female) learning disorder. (RAS). ADD/WO: n = 10 (8 male, 2 female) Exclusions: 3. RAN/RAS Combined Naming. 1. FSiQ < 70. 4. Other neuro!ogica! disorder. Operational Criteria: 1. WISGR: FSIQ > 70. 2. K-SADS: parent and teacher. 3. Parent and teacher behavior rating scales (Achenbach; SNAP). Note: ANCOVA = analysis of covariance; ERP = event-related potential; LNNB-CR = Luria-Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery-Children's Revision; RD = reading disabled; RT = reaction time; WRAT = Wide Range Achievement Test. D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 terviews and rating scales. Cohen's kappas were cal- culated to determine the reliability of these diagnos- tic procedures. These reliability measures ranged from .77 to .80 for both ADD/H and ADD/WO subjects. The studies of Sergeant and Scholten (1985) and Hynd, Lorys, et al. (1991) also employed similar multimodal identification procedures. This approach to ADD/H and ADD/WO identi- fication represents a vast improvement over the unidimensional assessment evident in most of the behavioral studies. The approach also has demon- strated adequate reliability (Spitzer et al., 1967). On the other hand, two difficulties with the approach are apparent. First, as mentioned previously, in the Hynd et al. (1989) study, these operational criteria resulted in one third of the A D D N O subjects hav- ing overlapping conduct disorders, which is con- trary to the typical behavioral profile for A D D N O . Second, no specific criteria for rating-scale usage were given in any of the studies. If, for example, the SNAP criteria from the Carlson et al. (1986) behav- ioral study were applied here, then hyperactivity indicators were permitted among ADD/WO sub- jects, which may account for some of the ADD/WO overlap with conduct disorders in Hynd et al. (1989). Another approach to ADD subtyping appears in the neuropsychological studies of Holcomb et al. (1985, 1986) and Trommer, Hoeppner, Lorber, and Armstrong (1988). Teacher ratings on the Ab- breviated Conners (Sprague et al., 1974) or Parent Conners rating scales and clinical rating by a psychi- atric evaluation team were used to classify ADD subgroups. Several problems are apparent with this approach. First, measurement is unidimensional. Second, there are several concerns about the Ab- breviated Conners Rating Scale (mentioned earlier). Holcomb et al. (1985, 1986),however, do attempt to address some of those concerns by specifying criteria with the Conners other than a total score. Third, measures of the reliability of the psychiatric evalua- tion team are not reported. Dependent Variables The final methodological issue related to the neu- ropsychological studies is what measures have been employed to differentiate ADD/H from ADD/WO children. Ten different measures have been used: visual and auditory event-related potentials (Hol- comb et al., 1985, 1986), the Luria-Nebraska Neu- ropsychological Battery-Children's Revision (LNNB-CR, Lorys et al., 1990; Schaughency et al., 1990), sustained attention measures (Stone, 1986), reaction-time measures (Hynd et al., 1989), high- speed visual search tasks (Sergeant & Scholten, 1985), rapid naming tasks (Hynd, Lorys, et al., 296 1991), an auditory go-no-go paradigm (Trommer, Hoeppner, Lorber, & Armstrong 1988), word re- trieval tasks (Goodyear, 1990), and a reading skill analysis (Goodyear, 1990). Some electrophysiologi- cal and neurocognitive differences have emerged. To summarize regarding methodological issues of neuropsychological studies, better experimental de- sign and use of multimodal assessment represent major improvements over the methodological prob- lems of behavioral studies. However, many of the other criticisms of methods in the behavioral studies apply to the neuropsychological studies as well. Inattention to status of medication must be consid- ered a serious issue when recording neurocognitive measures. Further concerns about co-occurring diagnoses have been raised. In addition, incomplete or impure measurement of DSM-III criteria con- tinue to be problems. Finally, the inconsistency of operational ddnitions, especially of ADD/WO, is problematic. Neuropsychological Differentiation of ADD/H and ADD/WO Next, consideration is given to the results of the neuropsychological attempts to distinguish ADD/H and ADD/WO. Additionally, differences between ADD/WO subjects and controls from this research are noted. Sixty percent of the neuropsychological studies have yielded significant differences between ADD/H and ADD/WO. Holcomb et al. (1986) found that ADD/H and ADD/WO subjects could be distinguished on the p 2 component of an auditory event-related potential. The amplitude of P 2 for ADD/H children was greater than for A D D N O children. The p 2 component is thought to measure the auditory nerve volley (Allison, 1984). For an auditory stimulus as in the Holcomb et al. (1986) study, the P component has a single fronto-central focus (Ritter, Kelso, Kutas, & Shiffrin, 1984) which was larger for ADD/H than for ADD/WO subjects in this study. Comparison to controls in the neuropsychologi- cal studies demonstrates that children with ADD/ WO, as well as children with ADD/H, have smaller than normal P 3 components in both auditory and visual modalities, and longer P3 latencies to both target and novel stimuli. P is thought to be closely related to attention, especially to the "relevance" of the eliciting stimulus (Picton, Donchin, Ford, Kahneman, & Norman, 1984). It is typically largest over the central (C,) and parietal (P,) areas. Neuropsychological distinctions among ADD subtypes were also found by Hynd, Lorys, et al. (1991). They found that ADD/WO children were slower than ADD/H children in responding to tasks D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 SUBTYPES OF ADD requiring them to name familiar alternating stimuli (e.g., colors, numbers, letters) as fast as possible. They interpreted these findings as consistent with a high rate of co-occurring learning disabilities, as learning-disabled children are thought to have defi- cits in automaticity (Hynd & Willis, 1988). This is also consistent with Ackerman, Anhalt, Holcomb, and Dlykrnan (1986) who hypothesized a lack of autoxnaticity of math facts for ADD/WO children based on latencies in solving math facts. Thase re- sults may also support previous behavioral findings by Lahey et al. (1985) of sluggish tempo among children with ADDJWO. Lorys et al. (1990) also found neurocognitive distinctions between ADD/ WO and ADD/H groups. They showed that ADD/ WO subjects demonstrate sensory-localization defi- cits, which are thought to be associated with parietal lobe functioning. Specifically, they found that per- formance on a finger agnosia task for the-left hand discriminated the ADD groups. Trommer, Hoeppner, Lorber, and Armstrong (1988) provided further support for cognitive dif- ferentiation of ADD/H and ADDJWO. They found significant differences between the ADD groups in error 11attems on a paradigm designed to evaluate the ability to emit a simple motor response to one stimulus and inhibit the response to a similar stimu- lus. ADD/WO subjects were initially more impul- sive than A.DD/H subjects and normals in the first trial bnt, unlike the ADD/H group, showed signifi- cant improvement in the second trial. ADDJH sub- jects were consistently impulsive in both trials. Sev- eral interpretations of these results are possible. First, ithis pattern of performance is consistent with the idea of a sustained attention disorder in ADD/H and a sielective attention disorder in ADDJWO. Sec- ond, PkDDJWO has beep linked consistently with anxiet:y in the behavioral studies through self-report and family history. It is possible that the initially impulsiive performance in the first trial is related to an initial nonfaciliatory level of performance anxiety which dissipates with continued performance. Oo~~dyear (1990) found further neurocognitive distinctions on word retrieval measures and on spe- cific rtmding skills. Interestingly, both groups had similar total scores on a word retrieval task but demonstvated qualitatively different performances. The A.DDtl,rWCJ group required significantly more word retrieval cues to achieve a performance similar to thalt of the A D D m group. Such a finding sug- gests s'ignifi~cant limitations for traditional psycho- metrics in distinguishing these groups. Additionally, children vvith ADDJWO, unlike those with ADD/%, slhowed an inconsistent pattern in the de- velopnient {of reading skills with word-attack skills exceeding word identification skills. Altlhough no ADD/H-ADD/WO differences emerged from Sergeant and Scholten's (1985) study, a distinction between ALDD/WO subjects and nor- mal controls was noted. Although this finding should receive cautious interprietation due to the fact that the ADD/WO group in this study was experi- mentally rather than clinically defined, ADD/WO subjects were slower than controls in the search stage of information processing. Sergeant and Scholten thought this findin4 was consistent with the presence of a selectivie attention deficit in this group, a hypothesis that has r&eive:d recent support from Barkley et al. (1990) Thle slo~wer rate of infor- mation processing may also be colnsistent with the Lahey et al. (1985) ancl Hynd Lorys, et al. (1991) results demonstrating sluggish cognitive tempo in ADD/WO children. Thus far, neuropsycbologi+al attempts to differ- entiate ADD/H from A.DD/YVO have yielded some early disappointing rewxlts but some recent encour- aging findings. Some possible reasons for this early lack of differentiation in th/e neiuropsychological studies may include the: follo$ing. First, it must be recognized that there are n$menous clinical and msthodological criticisms of the neiuropsychological studies. These problems- hich include small ADD/WO samples, ADD /IT 0 overlap with con- duct disorders, possibl~e incljsion of hyperactivity indicators in ADDJWlO ~defi ition~s, some inatten- tion to medication status, an utilization of cogni- I the dependent measures of limited complexity- may have contributed lo the tack of success in this research. Of particular1no1.e is limited task ciomplex- ity, which ranged from 11 tasks at the sim- plest lievel, to tasks of m mediate level, to rapid 1 level, The low levels oft by recognition and mr sufficient to elicit ADID/H a/nd A,DD/WO differ- ences in neurocog~tive: procftsses. Second, Schaughency et al. (1990) suggested that the construct validity of the YNNB-CR may be an important issue in their lac4 of IIDDJH-ADD/ WO differential~ion. Factor a alysis of the LNNB- CR with learning-disabled s udelnts by Snow and Hynd (1985) indicated Ihe we 1 kest factor was a Sen- sory-Motor factor. Early behavioral results with ADD subtypes as well aa; fin 'ngs; by Lorys et al. P (1990) suggest that it wchuld be tlhis factor rather than Verbal-Intelligence or deneral Academic fac- tors (the two other factors of tve LINNB-CR) which might distinguish ADD/H fkom ADD,fWO chil- dren. Additionally, ScIiaugh$ncy et al. (1990) re- ported that although deficits on which ADD sub- types might be differentiated are consistent with dysfunctional frontal-lobe develc~pment, frontal- lobe tasks were eliminated &om the LNNB-CR battery based on the assumption lhat behaviors as- D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 SUBTYPES OF ADD If there are two distinct disorders sharing compo- nent(~) of attention in their clinical features, then how might the two be characterized? Douglas and Peters ((1979) described their view of ADD children diagnosed early as central nervous system ADDs who have impaired input processes. This is contra- dicted by Sagvolden and Archer (1989) and by Le- vine, C'ary, Crocker, and Gross (1983) who suggest ADD difficulties may be output problems. Levine et al. described children with developmental output failure who acquire input (especially reading skills) adequaiely but experience academic deterioration when demands for written output occur later in ele- mentary and middle school. It makes some concep- tual sense that ADD/H children who are identified somewhat earlier may have input difficulties possi- bly rel,ated to behavioral problems associated with activity level and/or impulsivity whereas ADD/ WO children who are identified somewhat later may manifest output difficulties related to neurocognitive procesrses associated with dysfunction of automa- tized isformation processing and slow cognitive speed. Such a conceptualization of A D D N O is as appeali~ng as it is parsimonious, allowing for the inte- gration of key neurocognitive deficits (i.e., long-term ret~iev,al of verbal information, lack of automaticity with number facts, slow writing speed, etc.) emerg- ing from this review. On the other hand, Douglas and Peters (1979) argued that disrupted input processes can have spi- raling effects due to the impact an metacognitive praces!ses. There is evidence to support such an oc- currence vvith ADD from Nussbaum, Grant, Roman, Paole, and Bigler (1990); however, the sig- nificance of their research findings is undermined by their f'ailure to subgroup ADDs. Such evidence would suggest, however, that output difficulties of ADD/WO children as described by Levine et al. (1983) may result from antecedent disturbances of selective attention. Research, especially linking these irnues with research in cognitive psychology, is needed1 to resolve some of these questions. Recently, research has revealed that diagnoses of leaxning disabilities frequently co-occur among ALID/'WO but not necessarily among ADDJH chil- dren (ldynd, Loqs, et al., 1991). This possible link between ADD/WO and lewning disabilities has previoilsly been suggested by Maurer and Stewart (1980),, over half of whose ADD/WO case reviews had co-o~curring learning disabilities, and by B. A. Shaywbt~ ( 1987), who reported that an estimated 10% olf children with ADD are also learning dis- abled. The consistency of these recent findings with the coxwergence of the empirical results of behav- ioral and neuropsychological studies suggests that fertile research areas for the differential diagnosis of ADD/WO or WADD be in attentionalJcognitive and learning disabilities areas. It allso suggests, con- trary to the conclusion of Carlson let al. (1986), that ADD,IH and ADD/WC) differences are both behav- ioral and cognitive despite earlier difficulties in es- tablishing differences on traditiord psychometric measures (Hynd & Willis, 1988). The notion that learning disabilities frequently co-occur with ADD hats been incorporated1 in the most recent federal definition of learning disabilities (Wyngaarden, 1987). It may be that ADD/WO is the most frequent concurrent tp learning disabilities, not ADD/H. Future reseaxchneeds to address this issue as the preliminary stuqies dliscussed in this review suggest that children @th A.DDA3 rnay not do poorly in academic attainment, leven though their school performance may be variable. The relation between AD@/A.IDHD (or ADD subtypes) and learning di~~abilities is extremely im- portant (S. E. Shaywitz Pr. Bl A. Shaywitz, 1988; Silver, 1990; Wyngaarden, 19/87). IBoth the DSM- 111 and DSM-111-R note tHe fre:quent co-occur- rence of ADD and ADHD, re~pecltilvely, with learn- ing problems but so far t he exapt incidence or nature of comorbidity has not been eptabhished. This issue is highlighted by the publica ion of recent studies (e.g., r4ugust & Garfinkel, 199 ) in which there is an unquestioning acceptance of 1 he JWM-III-R no- menclature. The failure to exaqhine: the nature of the symptoms that allow for a di~gnotsis of ADHD in relation to the co-occurrence o/f academic difficulties significantly diminishes tlhe dontmibution of these studies. Although the studies by ~ ~ n d , Lorys, et al. (199 1); Barkley et al. (1 990); dnd FdIaurer and Stew- art (1980) suggest a link bet+eer~ ADD/VV'O and learning disabilities, the qu~esti n remains as to what is the relation between ADD ! I3 and academic un- deracliievement? A recent study by Nieves, Connor, Hynd, Lahey, and Town (id press) directly ad- dresses this issue. Based on th literature suggesting that conduct disorder and A. 4 D/'l3 frequently co- occur and that children with cbnduct disorder suffer academic problems (Riltter, izar~d, & Whitmore, 1970; Wilson & Herrnstein, 1 f 851, Nieves et al. (in press) examined undera~chieve@enit in children care- fully diagnosed as having oqly c:c)nduct disorder, children diagnosed as having eonduct disorder with co-occurring ADD/H, and ~hkldreim in a cluiic con- trol population. The findings 01 this study confirmed the fact that conduct disordkr is more often as- sociated with lower verbal IQ n d Ibat children with conduct disorder and ADD/$ tend to have lower academic achievement, particplarli~r in reading and spelling. The deficit in achievement, although statistically significantly below IQ, vvas just beyond the standard error of measurement (SE,) for IQ. This "deficit" D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 in academic attainment in conduct-disordered chil- dren with ADD/H would not be considered as ap- proximating the significant discrepancy between measured IQ and academic achievement normally required for a diagnosis of learning disabilities (Wyngaarden, 1987). Also, in the Nieves et al. (in press) study, socioeconomic status (SES) was sig- nificantly lower in the children diagnosed as having conduct disorder with co-occurring ADD/H. This finding is relevant because learning disabilities should not be diagnosed if the deficit in achievement is linked to SES factors (Wyngaarden, 1987). Thus, it could be concluded that underachieve- ment in ADD/H is related to the co-occurrence of lower verbal IQ in conduct disorder possibly linked to SES and is not characteristicof the considerably more significant delays in academic achievement evidenced by children diagnosed as having ADD/ WO. This tentative conclusion is supported by the finding of Hynd, Lorys, et al. (1991) that children with A D D R O , not ADD/H children, had au- tomatized linguistic deficits thought to characterize children with learning disabilities (Hynd 4 Semrud- Clikeman, 1989; Stanovich, 1986; Wolt Bally, & Morris, 1986). Other issues further cloud a more meaningful articulation of the relations among deficient aca- demic achievement, diagnosed learning disabilities, and ADD subtypes. For example, in both DSM-IiI and DW-IIJ-R, the diagnostic criteria for devel- opmental learning disorders have been criticized for their lack of clarity and failure to signify what con- stitutes a delay (Hoaper, Hynd, & Mattison, 1992). The DSM nomenclature is not in keeping with tradi- tional educational diagnostic practice as to the se- vere deficit in achievement (e.g., > 15 to 20 stan- dard score points) &at is typically required for a diagnosis of learning disabilities. Other important issues exist. For example, fac- tor-analytic studies suggest that attention deficit dis- order may be nanif~sted in two ways: (a) attention- cognitive (Berry et al., 1985) or inattention-disorganized (Lahey et al., 1988), and (b) attention-behavioral (Berry et al., 1985) or motar-impulsivity (Lahey et al., 1988). Although it is tempting to think that symptoms associated with ADD cluster into two broad factors, studies investigating the relation between conduct disorder and ADDkH suggest that three subgroups may exist: aggressive, h~peractive, and aggressive- hyperactive (Hinshaw, 1987; Szatmari, Boyle, & Offord, 1989'). Thus, behavioral symptoms as- soclatted with aggrtission and hyperactivity may be separable as might the symptoms associated with ADD and learning disability. Based on these find- ings w d the preceding discussion, it seems reason- able to ask if ADCI/H and ADD/WO are indeed subtypes of ADD or are they two separate syn- dromes sharing deficits in some components of at- tention? Barkley et al. (1990, 1991) suggest the latter because <f qualitatively different attentional styles found in the two groups. To date, only Barkley et al. (1990) have addressed this issue in the context of examining the kinds of behaviorally defined symptoms of inattention in children with ADD/H and those with ADD/WO who have been placed in learning disabilities class- rooms. As an example, it may well be that children with ADD/H have symptoms of inattention en- dorsed by parents or teachers that more closely re- flect impulsivity (e.g., frequent shifts from one un- completed activity to another) rather than those reflecting the kind of inattention symptoms one might expect from a ADD/WO child with co-occur- ring learning disabilities (e.g., often loses things nec- essary for tasks or activities at school or at home). The factor-analytic studies by Berry et al. (1985) and Lahey et al. (1988) suggest this issue is meaningful; findings in this regard may assist in addressing whether there are related subtypes or separate forms of childhoad psychopathology with different mani- festations of inattention. As Ingram (1990) sug- gested, there are many components to attention and deficits in various aspects of attentian may manifest differently in certain forms of childhood psychopa- thology. The neuropsychological studies, although few in number and characterized by methodological prob- lems, are important because they attempt to articu- late etiological, neurocognitive relations that the be- havioral-descriptive studies cannot address. It is clear, however, that studies conducted to date have, by and large, employed neuropsycho10~ical mea- sures of questionable validity or relevance (e.g., Hynd et al., 1989; Lorys et al., 1990; Schaughency et al., 1990; Sergeant & Scholten, 1985; Stone, 1986). Theoretically, many of these studies have been based on the literatwe suggmting thal; children with ADD/H have deficits in attention and motor regula- tion similar to patients with frontal-lobe lesions (Chelune, a erg us on, Koon, & Dickey, 1986; Drewe, 1975; Mattes, 1980). Indeed, most neurobiological models of ADD/H implicate dysgnction in dience- phalic-fordbrain inhibitory systems (Ximetkin & Rapoport, 1986). Recent rolrjjrvndl aerebrd 1 Blood flow/co~~puted tomography (rCBP/GT) studies suppart this concoptpalirzation as the caudata-stria- tal region seems hypoactiva in ADDJH children when compwed to controls (Lou, Henrik'sen, & Bruhn, 1984; Lou, Manriks~b, Bruhn, Bomer, & Nielsen, 19891. Magqetic rewamce imaging; (MR.1) also has been employed to irsv~stigate dedaticrils in brain mlorphpllagy in ADD/H children; preliminary findings indiaata that the frontal lobes are character- D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 SUBTYPES OF ADD ized by symmetry as opposed to the normal pattern of right frontal width greater than left frontal width (Hynd,, Semrud-Clikeman, Lorys, Novey, & Eli- opulos, 1990). Variation in other subcortical struc- tures may also exist. Exploratory research has sug- gested that the genu and splenium of the corpus callosum may be smaller in ADD/H (and ADHD) children when compared to matched controls (Hynd,, Setnrud-Clikeman, et al., 1991). This varia- tion also may relate to the allocation of hemispheric attentional resources (Hynd, Sernrud-Clikeman, et al., 1991). 141~0, Hynd et al. (in press) using MRI have reported that chiidren with ADHD (all sub- jects miet criteria for ADD/H) have reversed asym- metry ~Qeft less than right) of the caudate nucleus, a subcortica1 structure with rich connections to the froptal lobes. This finding may be correlated with those c~f LOU et al. (1984, 1989). Consequently, evi- dence from a neurological perspective does suggest that anitmior dysfunction in structures involving at- tention~ and motor inhibition may be associated with ADD/H. Further, allocation of hemispheric atten- tional resources may be affected in ADDRI. Schiaughency and Hynd (1989) extended neuro- logical conceptualization in this regard to account for the behavioral symptomatology of ADD/WO. They proposed that the behaviors associated with ADD/WO seem more reflective of posterior, possi- bly right-hemispheric dysfunction. The sluggish cognitive tempo, frequently noted problems in achievement, particularly arithmetic, and possible visuospatial problems may reflect posterior dysfunc- tion. The studies by Schaughency et al. (1990); Lorys let al. (1990); and Hynd, Lorys, et al. (1991) examined these possible relations. Unfortunately, two of these studies employed tests or ~ s k s from the LNNB-CR which failed to find predicted deficits other than rthe fact that the ADD/WO children in Lorys et al. (1990) did show sensory-localization deficits compared to the ADD/H group. Concur- rent tal thme studies (Lorys et al., 1990; Schaug- hency et al., 19912), research accumulated indicating that the LNNB-CR was not particularly useful in clinical differentiation of children with significant psychopathology or developmental disorders (Hynd, 1988). Future studies should incoxporate develo~~mentally apprspriata task more sensitive to the neurobehavioral manifestations of ADD/H rather than employ clinical-psychometric measures of cognitive-perceptual processes thought to be cor- related to anterior or posterior dysfunction. For example, based on neurological evidence sug- gesting that the right hemisphere is important in mediatmg attention (Heilman & Van Den Abell, 1980; Iteivicch, Alavi, & Gur, 1984), Voeller and Hleilman (1988a) examined children with ADHD on a letter and line cancellation tasks. Similar to pa- tients with known riglht-hemisph~eric lesions, the ADHD children evidenced more cancellation fail- ures onthe left side of tlhe page. It also appears that ADHD children evidence deficits in the ability to sustailn a simple motor act (motlor irnpersistence; Voeller & Heilman, 1980b), a deficit more frequently associated with right front$l lelsions in adults (Kertesz, Nicholson, & (Canceoiere:, 1985). Perform- ance on tasks more sensitive to disrupted neurologi- cal systems, such as those used by Yoeller and Heil- man (1988a, 1988b), may prcjve Inore meaningful, for example, than tests from the LINNB-GR in in- vestigating the notion that @vergent hemispheric systems may be differentially affected in A,DD/H and ALDD/WO (Schaughenc$ & Hynd, 1989). Conclusion Can it be concludecl that there: are two ADD subtypes? At present, our refiew does not permit such a categorical answer. The behavioral studies do provide good evidence in supgjort of the notion that at least two subtypes or two disorders may exist. However, as noted previously/ the total number of ADD,/WO subjects included ilp thes;e studies may be quite small thus calling into qkestion the reliability of the findings of these studieq. Also, the neuropsy- chological studies provide onlp limited support for clierentiating ADD/H and AT)D/WO. Signifi- cantly though, the instrament$ employed to investi- gate possible differences seem 10 cloud the interpre- tation of the studies that r e k t negative results (Hyncl et al., 1989; Lorys et a I., 1990; Schaughency et al., 1990; Stone, 1986). Fina \ ly, there may well be significant age-related elFects ob neuropsychological variables as suggested by ~ec)ter et al. (1987) and Passler et al. (1986) with regarfl to frontal-lobe mea- sures that obscure differential @erfo~rmance by ADD subtypes on other neuropsyc ological measures. Generally, however, this 1 r view provides more suppart for the diagnostic appdoaclh found in DSM- III (American Psychiatxic Mociation, 1 980) than that outlined in DSM-IJY- R (hmerican Psychiatric Association, 1987). In fact, the ondifferentiated list- ing of 14 possible symptorns 4 r PLDHD in DSM- 111-R seems clearly at 7sarianGe with the decades of research emphasizing the pre omdnance of symp- toms associated with motor'ov ractivity. Four to 5 of ! the 14 symptoms listed emphaqize inattention (New- corn et al., 1989) whereas onlp 3 dlenote behaviors specifically considered reflective of hyperactivity (see Table 1). Consequently, 6 sy~dptmis in the DSM- III-R denote behaviors associpted with impulsivity which factor-analytic sttldies hbve clearly shown are split between two larger factoi-s co~mprising behav- isrs associated with eitller idttention or hyperac- tivity (Berry et al., 1985; Lahey et al., 1988). D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 Thus, although the research conducted to date does not permit us to state conclusively that there are two ADD subtypes or that there are two sepa- rate syndromes each sharing behaviorally defined deficits in attention, the research does provide more support for the diagnostic approach found in DSM- IIL In other words, there is more support for the differentiation of behavioral symptoms into those classified as inattentive or hyperactive than there is for a polythetic listing of behaviors cutting across all three domains (inattention, impulsivity, hyperac- tivity), any combination of which might result in a diagnosis. The polythetic approach results in a very heterogeneous group of subjects (August & Garfin- kel, 1990; Newcorn et al., 1989). Further behavioral studies are needed to address the many methodological problems characterizing the literature to date if a more theoretically and clinically meaningful differentiation of the ADD phenotype(s) is to be derived. As there appear to be significant family-genetic risk factors in ADD (Bied- erman, Faraone, Keenan, Knee, & Tsuang, 1990), the careful empirical validation of the DSM ap- proach should be a priority in research if the neuro- logical-genetic nature of this behaviorally diagnosed disorder is to be achieved. Likewise, more conceptu- ally sound neurological/neuropsychological studies are needed that tiit? dysfunctional process in children with ADD to deviatibns in brain metabolic pro- cessles or brain rnorphol~gy. Only in this fashion will data from the behavioral sciences significantly in- fluence the ability of researchers in the neuro- sciences ta articulate more fully the neurobiological basis and treatment d this common behavioral dis- order. References Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. S. (1983). Manual for the teacher's report form and teacher version of the Child Behav- ior Profile. Burlington, VT: University Associates in Psychi- atry. Ackerman, P. T., Anhalt, J. M., Dykman, R. A., & Holcomb, P. J. (1986). Effortful processing deficits in children with read- ing and/or attention disorders. Brain and Cognition, 5, 22- 40. Ackerrnan, P. T., Anhalt, J. M., Holcomb, P. J., & Dykman, R. A. (1986). Presumably innate and acquired automatic pro- cesses in children with attention and/or reading disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 513-529. Allison, T. (1984). Recording and interpreting event-related po- tentials. In E. Donohin (Ed.), Cognitive psychophysiology: Event-related potentials and the study of cognition (pp. 63- 97). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. American Psychiatric Association. (1968). Diagnostic and statis- tical manual of mental disorders (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statis- tical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statis- tical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washing- ton, DC: Author. August, G. J., & Garfinkel, B. D. (1989). Behavioral and cogni- tive subtypes of ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 739-748. August, G. J., & Garfinkel, B. D. (1990). Comorbidity of ADHD and reading disability among clinic-referred children. Jour- nal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18, 29-45. Barkley, R. A. (1982). Guidelines for defining hyperactivity in children. In B. B. Lahey & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in clinical child psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 137-180). New York: Plenum. Barkley, R. G., DuPaul, G. J., & McMurray, M. B. (1990). Comprehensive evalu$tion of attention deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity as defined by research criteria. Journal of Consultingand Clinical Psychology, 58, 775-789. Barkley, R. A., DuPaul, G. J., & McMurray, M. B. (1991). Attention deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity: Clinical response to three doses of methylphenidate. Pediat- rics, 87, 519-531. Becker, M. G., Isaac, W., & Hynd, G. W. (1987). Neuropsycho- logical development of non-verbal behaviors attributed to "frontal lobe"' functioning. Developmental Neuropsychology, 3, 275-298. Berry, C. A., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1985). Girls with attention deficit disotder: A silent minority? A report on behavioral and cognitive characteristics. Pediatrics, 76, 801-809. Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Keenan, K., Knee, D., & Tsuang, M. T. (1990). Family-genetic and psychosocial risk factors in DSM 111 attention deficit disorder. Journal of the Ameri- can Academy af Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 526- 533, Borden, K. A,, Brown, R. T., & Clingerman, S. R. (1985). Valid- ity of attention deficit disorder: A second look. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 55, 466-467. Brown, R. T. (1986). Teaohex ratings and the assessment of atten- tion detldt disordered children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 119, 95 - 100. Cantwell, D. P. (1984). The attention deficit disorder syndrome: Current knowledge, future trends. Journal of American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 23, 3 15-3 18. Cantwell, D. P., & Baker, L. (1988).Issues in the classification of child and adolescent psychopathology. Journal of the Americm Academy of Chjld and Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 521-533. Carlson, C. L, (1986). Attention deficit disorder without hyperac- tivity: A review of preliminary expedmental evidence. In B. B. Lahey & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in clinical child psycholap (Vol. 9, pp. 153-176). New York: Plenum. Carlson, C. L., Lahey, B. B,, Frame, C. L., Walker, J., & Hynd, G. W. (191817). Sociom~tria status of clinic-referred children with attention deficit disorders with and without hyperac- tivity. Journal ofAbnormol Child Psychology, 1.5. 537-547. Carlson, C. L., M e y , B. B., & Neeper, R. (1986). Direct assess- mleht of the cognitive, corrqlatas of attention deficit disorders with and without hyperactivity. Journal of Psy~hopathology and Behavi~ml Assessment, 8, 69 - 86. Chelune, G. J., Fe~guson, W., Koon, R., 8r; Dickey, T. 0. (1986). Frontal lobe &isinhibition $I attention deficit disorde~. Child Psychiatq and Raman Pbvelopment, 16, 22 1-234. Cohen, M., 8L Hynd, G. W. (1986). The Connors Teacher Rating Scale: A Wereht fq~tor ~tructure with special education childrep. Psyc/~ology in the Schools, 23, 13-23, Conners, C. $C. (1969). A teadher rating scale for use in drug studies with children. ~merican Journal of Psychiatry, 126, 884-888. D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 SUBTYPES OF ADD Conte, R.., Kinsbourne, M., Swanson, J., Zirk, H., & Samuels, M. (1986). Presentation rate effects on paired associate learning by attention deficit disordered children. Child Development, 57, 681-687. Denckla, M . IB., & Heilrnan, K. M. (1979). The syndrome of hyperactivity. In K. M. Heilman & E. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology (pp. 233-284). New York: Oxford University Press. Douglas, V. I., & Peters, K. G. (1979). Toward a clearer defini- tioin of the attentional deficit of hyperactive children. In G. A. Hale L M. Lewis (Eds.), Attention and cognitive develop- ment (pp. 173-248). New York: Plenum. Drewe, IE. A. (1975). Go-no go learning after frontal lobe lesions in humans. Cortex, 11, 8- 16. Dykman, R. A., Ackerman, P. T., & McCray, D. S. (1980). Effects of methylphenidate on selective and sustained atten- tio~n in hyperactive, reading disabled, and presumably atten- tion-disordered boys. Journal of Nervous and Mental Dis- ease, 168, 745-752. Edelbrock, C., Costello, A. J., & Kessler, M. D. (1984). Empirical corroboration of attention deficit disorder. American Acad- emy of Child Psychiatry, 23, 285-290. Golden, C. J. (1981). The Luria-Nebraska Children's Battery: Theory and formulation. In G. W. Hynd & J. E. Obrzut (Etis.), Meumpsychological assessment and the school-age chlld: Issues and procedures (pp. 277-302). New York: Grune & Stratton. Goodyeiu, P. IR. (1990). Attention deficit disorders with and with- out hyperactivity: Neuropsychological processes. Unpub- lished doctoral dissertation, University of Georgia, Athens. Heilman, K. M., & Vaa Den Abell, T. (1980). Right hemisphere doiminance for attention: The mechanism underlying hemi- spheric asymmetries of inattention (neglect). Neurology, 30, 32'1-330. Hinshavr, S. P. (1987). On the distinction between attentional deficitshyperactivity and conduct problems/aggression ~n child psychopathology. Psycholqgical Bulletin, 101, 443- 463. Holcomb, P. J., Ackerman, P. T., & Dykman, R. A. (1985). Cognitive event-related brain potentials in children with attention and reading deficits. Psychophysiology, 22, 656- 66'1. Holcomb, P. J., Ackerman, P. T., $ Dykman, R. A. (1986). Auditory event-related potentials in attention and reading disabled boys. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 3, 26.3-273. Hooper, S., Wynd, G. W., & Mattison, R. (Eds.). (1992). Develop- mental disorders: Diagnostic criteria and clinical assessment. Hilllsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Hynd, I[>. W. (1988). Neuropsychological assessment in clinical chrld psychology. Newbvry Park, CA: Sage. Hynd, C ; . W., Hern, K., Leonard, T., Novey, E. S., Eliopulos, D., Mslrshall, R., & Gonzalaz, J. (in press). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and asymmetry of the caudate nu- cleus. Jo~urnal of Child Neurology. Hynd, C;. W., Lorys, A. R,, Semrud-Clikeman, M., Nieves, N., Huettner, M. I. S., & Lahey, B. B. (1991). Attention deficit disorder without hyperactivity: A distinct behavioral and neurocognitive syndrome. Journal of Child Neurology, 6, 35-411. Hynd, Ci. W., Nieves, N., Connor, R. T., Stone, P. A,, Town, P., Bel:ker, hd. G., Lahey, B. B., & Lorys, A. R. (1989). Atten- tiolrl deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity: Reac- ti0111 time and speed of cognitive processing. Journal of Lerlrning Disabilities, 22, 573-580. Hynd, C i . W., & Semrud-Clikeman, M. (1989). Dyslexia and brain morphology. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 447-482. Hynd, Gr. W., Semrud-Clikeman, M., Lorys, A. R., Novey, E. S., & Eliopulos, D. (1990). Brain morphology In developmental dyslexia and attention deficit disorder/hyperactivity. Ar- chives of Neurology, 47, 9 19-926. Hynd, G. W., Semrud-Clikemian, M., Lorys, A. R., Novey, E. S., Eliopulos, D., & Lyytinen, H . (19911). Corpus callosum morphology in attention-deficrt hylperactivity disorder (PLDHD): Morphometric analysis OF MRI. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 24, 141 - 146. Hynd, G. W., & Willis, W. G. (1988). Pediatric neuropsychology. Orlando, FL: Grune & Stratton. Ingram, R. E. (1990). Self-focused attention1 in clinical disorders: Review and a conceptual model. ~sychological Bulletin, 10% 156-176. Kertesz, A., Nicholson, I., & Cancelliere, Pi. (1985). Motor im- persistence: A right hemisphere I synclrome. Neurology, 35, 662-666. King, C., & Young, R. D. (1982). Attentional deficits with and without hyperactivity: Teacher and peer perceptions. Jour- nal of Abnormal Child Psy~cholqgv, 10, 483-496. Lahey, B. B., Pelham, W. E., Schaughency, E. A., Atkins, M. S., MLurphy, H. A., Hynd, (3. IN., qusso, M., Hartdagen, S., & Lorys-Vernon, A. (1988). Dimsnsions, and types of atten- tion deficit disorder. Journal oA the llmerican Academy of Child and Adolescent P.rychiat& 2 7, 330-335. Lahey, B. B., Schaughency, IE. A., Frame, C. L., & Strauss, C. C. (1985). Teacher ratings of attention problems in children experimentally classified as exhibiting attention deficit dis- orders with and without hyperaqtivity. Journalof the Amer- ican Academy of Child ond ~ d o k s c e n t Psychiatry, 24, 613- 616. Lahey, B. B., Schaughency, E . A., ~ j n d , G. W., Carlson, C. L., & Nieves, N. (1987). P~ttentioq defic~t disorder with and without hyperactivity: C!omparis/on of behavioral character- isltics of clinic referred children. Vourrzal of American Acad- emy of Child and Adolescent P$chiatry, 26, 7 18-723. Lahey, B. B., Schaughency, IE. A., Spauss, C. C., & Frame, C. L (1984). Are attention deficit 1sord4:rs with and without P' . hyperactivity similar or dissimilar disorders? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adohzscent Psychiatry, 23, 302-309. Levine, M. D., Carey, W. B., Croc er, A,. C., & Gross, R. T. Y . (1983). Developmental-behavio~plpedzatrics. Philadelphia: Saunders. Lorys, A. R., Hynd, G. W., 8: Lahey, B. (1990). Do neurocogni- tive measures differentiate intten Ion deficit disorder (ADD) 4' . ' . with and without hyperactivity?Archzi~es of Clinical Neuro- psychology, 5, 1 19- 135. Lou, H. C., Henriksen, L., $ ~ r u h d P. (1984). Focal cerebral hypo-perfusion in chddren with L ysphasia and/or attention deficit disorders. Archives of Nebrology, 41, 825-829. Lou, H. C., Henriksen, L., Bruhn, P., Born~er, H., & Nielsen, J. B. (1989). Striatal dysfunction i attention deficit disorder. Aj~chives of Neurology, 46, 48- ! 2. Mattes, J. A. (1980). The role of f ontal lobe dysfunction in t . childhood hyperkinesis.Compyhenszve Psychiatry, 358- 369. Maurer, R. G., & Stewart, M. A. (19k0). Attention defic~t with- out hyperactivity in a child ps$chiatric clinic. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 41, 232-23 . Meents, C. K. (1989). Attenttion de cit disorder: A review of literature. Psychology in the Sc / ools, 26, 168-178. Newcom, J . H., Halperin, J. M., Heqley, J. M., O'Brien, J. D., Pascualvaca, D. M., Wolf, Id. E. Morganstein, A., Sharma, I V., & Young, J. G. (1989). Are ADIIIH and ADHD the same or different? Journal of the Arnerican Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiard 285, 734-738. Nieves, N., Connor, R. T., Hynd, G. jW., Lahey, B. B., &Town, P. (in press). Academic underacjhievennent and intellectual D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 GOODYE AR & HYND ability in children with conduct disorder (CD) and attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity (ADD/H). Journal of Learning Disabilities. Nussbaum, N. L., Grant, M. L., Roman, M. J., Poole, J. H., & Bigler, E. D. (1990). Attention deficit disorder and the me- diating effect of age on academic and behavioral variables. Journal of Developmentaland Behavioral Pediatrics, 11, 22- 26. Ostrom, N. N., & Jenson, W. R. (1988). Assessment of attention deficits in children. Professional School Psychology, 3, 253- 269. Passler, M., Isaac, W., & Hynd, G. W. (1986). Neuropsychologi- cal development of behavior attributed to frontal lobe func- tioning in children. Developmental Neuropsychology, I, 349-370. Pelham, W. E., Atkins, M. S., & Murphy, H. A. (1981). Attention deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity: Definitional issues and correlates. In W. E. Pelharn (Ed.), DSM 111 category of attention deficit disorders: Rationale, operational- ization and correlates (pp. 47-48). Los Angeles: American Psychological Association. Picton, T., Donchin, E., Ford, J., Kahneman, D., &Norman, D. (1984). Report of Panel 11: The ERP and decision and memory processes. In E. Donchin (Ed.), Cognitive psycho- physiology: Event-related potentials and the study of cogni- tion (pp. 179-201). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum As- sociates, Inc. Quay, H. C., & Peterson, D. R. (1983). Interim manual for the Revised Behavior Problem Checklist. Miami: Authors (at University of Miami). Quay, H. C., Routh, D. K., & Shapiro, S. K. (1987). Psychopa- thology of childhood: From description to validation. An- nual Review of Psychology, 338, 491 -532. Reivich, M., klavi, A., & Gur, R. C. (1984). Positron emission tomographic studies of perceptual tasks. Annals of Neurol- ogy, 15, 561-565. Ritter, W., Kalso, W., Kutas, M., & Shiffrin, R. (1984). Report of Panel 111: Preparatory processes. In E. Donchin (Ed.), Cognitive psychophysiology: Event-related potentials and the study of cagnition (pp. 214-263). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Ross, D. M., & Ross, S. A. (1982). Hyperactivity: Current issues, research and theory. New York: Wiley. Routh, D. K. (1988). Attention deficit disorder. In R. T. Brown & C. R. Reynolds (Eds.), Psycho1ogicalperspectives on child- hood e~ceptionality: A handbook (pp. 83- 119). New York: Wiley. Rubenstein, R. A., & Brown, R. T. (1984). An evaluation of the validity of the diagnostic category of attention deficit disorder. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 54, 398- 414. Rutter, M., Tizard, J., & Whitmore, K. (Eds.). (1970). Education, health, and behavior. New York: Wiley. Sagvolden, T., & Archer, T. (1989). Future perspectives on ADD research: An irresistible challenge. In T. Sagvolden & T. Archer (Eds.), Attention de$cit disorder: Clinical and basic research (pp. 123-154). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Schaughency, E. A., & Hynd, G. W. (1989). Attention and im- pulse contieol in attention deficit disorders (ADD): Neural control systems. Learning and Individual Dzfferences, 1, 423-449. Schaughency, E. A., Lahey, B. B., Hynd, G. W., Stone, P. A., & Piqcentini, J. C. (1990). Neuropsychological test perform- ance and ADD'S: Clinical utility of the Luria-Nebraska Neuropsychoiogical Battery-Children's Revision. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 1 12- 1 16. Sergeant, J. A,, & Scholten, C. A. (1985). On data limitations in hyperactivity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychzatry, 26, 111-124. Shaywitz, B. A. (1987). Hyperactivity/attention deficit disorder. In Interagency Committee on Learning Disabilities (Ed.), Learning disabilities: A report to the US. Congress (pp. 6 1 - 83). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Shaywitz, S. E., Schnell, C., Shaywitz, B. A., & Towle, V. R. (1986). Yale Children's Inventory: An instrument to assess children with attentional deficits and learning disabilities. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 14, 347-364. Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1988). Attention deficit dis- order: Current perspectives. In J. F. Kavanagh & T. J. Truss, Jr. (Eds.), Learning disabilities (pp. 369-523). Park- ton, MD: York. Silver, L. B. (1990). Attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder: Is it a learning disability or a related disorder? Journal of Learn- ing Disabilities, 23, 394-397. Snow, J. H., & Hynd, G. W. (1985). Factor structure of the Luria-Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery-Children's Revision with learning disabled children. Journal of School Psychology, 23, 27 1 - 276. Spitzer, R. L., Cohen, J., Fleiss, J. L., & Endicott, J. (1967). Quantification of agreement in psychiatric diagnosis. Ar- chives of General Psychiatry, 1 IZ 83-87. Sprague, R. L., Cohen, M. N., & Werry, J. S. (1974). Normative data on the Canners Teacher Rating and Abbreviated Scale (Tech. Rep.). Champaign: University of Illinois, Institute for Child Behavior and Development. Stanovitch, K. E. (1986). Cognitive processes and the reading problems of learning-disabled children: Evaluating the as- sumption of specificity. In J. Torgeson & B. Wong (Eds.), PsychoIogical and educatiorrul pergpectives on learning disabilities (pp. 87- 13 1). New Yark: Academic. Stone, P. A. (1986). Sustained attentian in children with attention dejcit disorder with and without hyperactivity. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Georgia, Athens. Szatmari, P., Boyle, M., & Offord, D. R. (1989). ADDH and conduct disorder: Degree of diagnostic overlap and differences mong correlates. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 865- 872. Trommer, B. L., Hoeppner, J. B., Lorber, R., & Armstrong, K. J. (1988). The go-no-go paradigm in attention deficit dis- order. Annqls of Neurology, 24, 610-614. Trommer, B. L., Hoepptler, J. B., Rosenberg, R. S., Armstrong, K. J., & Rothstein, J. A. (1988). Sleep disturbance in chil- dren with attantion deficit disorder [Abstract]. Annals of Neurology, 24, 322. Ullmann, R. K., Sleater, E. K., & Sprague, R. L. (1985). A change of mind: The Conners Abbreviated Rating Scales reconsidered. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 13, 553-565. Voeller, K. K. S., & Heilman, K. M. (1988a). Attentional deficit disorder in children: A neglect syndrome? Neurology, 38. 806-808. Voeller, K. K. S., & Heilman, K. M. (1988b). Motor impersist- ence in children with attentional deficit disorder: Evidence for right hemisphere dysfunction [Abstract]. Annals ofNeu- rology, 224, 323. Wechsler, D. (1974). Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Childen-Revised. New York: Psychological Corpora- tion. Werry, J. S., Sprague, R. L., & Cohen, M. N. (1975). Conners Teacher Rating Scale for use in drug studies with ch11- dren- An empirical study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy- chiatry, 3, 21 7-229. Wilson, J. Q., & Herrnstein, R. J. (1985). Crime and human nature. New York: Simon & Schuster. D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14 SUBTYPES OF ADD Wolf, M., Bally, H., & Morris, R.(1986). Automaticity, retrieval B. Lahey & A. Kazdin (Eds.), Advn~rces in clinical child processes, and reading: A longitudinal study in average and psychology (Vol. 9, pp. 177-216). New York: Plenum. imlpaired readers. Child Development, 57, 988- 1000. Wyngaarden, J. B. (Ed.). (1987). Learning disabilities: A report to the US. Congress. Washington, DC: Interagency Commit- tee on Learning Disabilities. Zametkin, A., & Rapport, J. (1986). The pathophysiology of Received No~ember 1, 1991 attlention deficit disorder with hyperactivity: A review. In B. Final revision received March 31, 1992 D ow nl oa de d by [ U Q L ib ra ry ] at 2 0: 05 1 6 N ov em be r 20 14